Abstract
The Humanities now confront a new era in cultural representations — the digital age.
As a consequence, our approach to culture may be modified because technology allows
us to now visualize our thoughts and theories using digital and computing techniques.
This research focuses on merging humanities research with computational sciences to
explore the processes involved in culture dynamics. We present an interdisciplinary
approach that combines literary studies, economics, and agent-based modeling (ABM)
and give details of how literature maybe used as a data set that can be translated
into a dynamic Java-based simulation of human interactions constructed around Game
Theory. Our model of Cross-Cultural Cooperation is designed to study culture at
various levels of granularity simultaneously in order to show how micro-behaviors
might lead to macro outcomes such as cultural group formation. We present one
experiment based on the literature of discovery and conquest in the U.S. Southwest
translated into the language of ABM. Additionally, we explore the role of space,
time, and population-size in this process and offer a discussion of possible future
directions for this type of research. The creation of our simulator of cultural
exchange between individuals of differing cultures allows researchers to experiment
with ideas about first and ongoing contact and speculate with “What if?”
scenarios.
Introduction
The humanities now confront a new era in cultural representations — the digital age.
As a consequence, our approach to culture may be modified because technology allows
us to now visualize our thoughts and theories using digital and computing techniques
[
Berry 2011, 1]. For this, we need an analytical framework that
integrates various disciplines synergistically. In this research, we offer the
possibility of complexity science and computer simulation as a means of accomplishing
this goal in the humanities.
This work focuses on merging humanities research with computational sciences to
explore the processes involved in culture dynamics. We present an interdisciplinary
approach that combines literary studies and agent-based modeling (ABM). The article
divides into five sections. In the first, we describe our approach to culture. Next,
we explain our “computational turn” by presenting the idea of agent-based
modeling in the humanities. In section three, we give details of how literature may
be used as a data set. We present our model and the results from one experiment using
our own ABM simulation in section four. And finally, we conclude with a discussion
and possible future directions for this type of research.
Hispanic Culture in ABM Perspective
The intent of our work is to better understand what behaviors give rise to diversity
within a cultural group, specifically the Hispanic culture as our area of research
interest. Dawkins (1978, 1999), Cavalli-Sforza and Feldman (1981), Cavalli-Sforza
(2000), and Sperber (2006, 2007) and others hold that that culture
[1] is a second process marching in step with
biological evolution. The evidence of this process might be viewed from a
materialistic position (Gabora 2007) such that, artifacts in the form of tools are
taken to indicate the existence of a “culture.” Consequently, if we follow the
evolution of “tools” in terms of their nature, number, and complexity, we have a
materialistic approach to culture and its development over time. Alternatively, we
might focus on the intangible aspects of culture such as the abstract systems of
language and behaviors (institutions) that transmit information between individuals
and groups of individuals, leading to learning over generations (Cavalli-Sforza and
Feldman 1981). These abstractions depend on mind processes such as awareness,
perception, judgment, and problem-solving [
Wilson and Keil 2001]. In this view, “Culture is the mental equipment that
society members use in orientating, transacting, discussing, defining,
categorizing, and interpreting actual social behavior in their society”
[
Wilson and Keil 2001].
[2]
When we add “Hispanic” to culture, we are limiting the view of culture to some
extent; however, there are diverse views on what Hispanic culture means. For some,
the Southwestern United States Borderlands embody the Hispanic, but why? Is it as
simple as the presence of the Spanish at some point? This simplification obscures the
fact that more complex processes have taken place over time between numerous
individuals and groups including but not limited to Anglo, Hispanic and Native
American. The complexity of the process is manifest in the definitions for the term
“Hispanic.” For example, the Real Academia Española notes that it often
refers to the populations of Hispania, the Roman name for the Iberian peninsula,
although the dictionary also notes that the term applies to the countries of
hispanoamérica and more specifically to individuals in the United
States whose origin is hispanoamérica [
Real Academia Española 2001]. Across the
Atlantic, in the late nineteenth century the term “hispanic” referred to the
Spanish-speaking New World [
Harper 2010–2012]. More recently, Hispanic is the
term used by U.S. government for census purposes to identify members of certain
groups (Cuban, Mexican, Puerto Rican, Central and South American [
US Bureau of the Census 1970]). The tag arises from a process of “self-identification”
based on a set of prompts such as “Mexican,
Puerto Rican, Cuban, Central or South American, or some other Hispanic
origin” (U.S. Bureau of the Census 1993). However, this Hispanic label
concerns various groups who feel that it does not adequately reflect the totality of
their experiences. Hence, we now see Latinos, Chicanos, Hispanos and other
identities. This diversity of self-identification and its expression as a group-level
phenomenon known as “culture” is at the very heart of our research.
The “Computational Turn”
Complexity or complex systems analysis is concerned with how the “simple”
organizes to become the complex. For example, how does a single cell become a brain?
How is it that a number of individuals acting in an individual way give rise to
larger social structures such as cultural groups, economies, financial or political
systems that span the globe? These systems seem to act in ways that are difficult to
perceive at certain levels of disaggregation (granularity). Additionally, their
emergence over time is equally difficult to predict, although from the perspective of
complexity science we can understand the possible systemic outcomes if we can
understand the nature of individual interactions within a certain context.
The starting point for this research is based on complexity; that is, to identify the
overarching system and then to further disaggregate this entity into its individual
parts such that we understand the correlation in the behaviors of individuals that
lead to a higher-order system. Individuals and their behaviors are the focus rather
than simply the individual as a unit in a linear system that adds two and two to
result in four. The rules of behavior between individuals are about the transmission
of information and are in fact very simple and limited. However, the emergent system,
as the interplay of individual behaviors is called, is believed to differ from the
sum of its parts, and the system itself changes over time. Mitchell offers as
examples of these types of systems a solar system, the brain, a stock market, and
global climate change [
Mitchell 2009, 16]. These are extremely
complex systems but when disaggregated into their parts planets, cells, individual
equity traders, and simple molecules are interacting with each other in a way not
clear to us if we take a systemic view. This is where complexity science differs from
traditional reductionist scientific analysis in its methodology and view. That is to
say, complexity science acknowledges there is no clear way to reduce a complex whole
into the sum of its parts because other forces must be at play between the parts.
However, we can take a small segment of this complexity and break it down into its
parts and rules that govern the behavior of these parts in order to better understand
how a dynamic system emerges with sometimes difficult to predict outcomes.
For our research, we consider “culture” a macro-level phenomenon that may be
visualized as a dynamic process using ideas from complexity in combination with new
computational technology. In order to arrive at this end, we combine “data” from
a set of cultural artifacts — written artifacts to be precise; (2) the concepts of
evolution and emergence from complexity; (3) a behavioral mechanism consistent with
culture but translatable to computer code — Game Theory; and (4) a method of encoding
and visualizing these ideas. In our case, the process of self-identification by
individuals is recast in terms of game theory in order to better understand how
individual choice leads to group-level diversity. This, we believe, means that a
group as it is broadly interpreted contains individuals that share a common process
of self-identification; however, when the level of granularity for
self-identification changes, suddenly not everyone in the group fits into the
previously clear-cut category as was once thought. This we believe approximates
emergence because it is an inevitable consequence of the expansion of simple rules
governing individual self-identification. By studying these ideas using complexity,
we hope to better understand how cultures emerge or evolve, rather than simply accept
their existence.
[3]
Literature as Data
Literary objects such as written texts are cultural objects that represent cultural
information. In this respect they might be viewed as material evidence of a culture’s
existence. In other words, they may be considered as either data points or sets
relating to a culture. For example, one could count the number of books (data points)
produced in a given space of time and assign a meaning to the output such that it
becomes a measure of cultural development expressed through book production. In fact,
the United Nations Education, Scientific and Cultural Organization (UNESCO) has taken
on the task of measuring culture through data such as book publications on a yearly
basis as one means of measuring the standard of living in a country [
United Nations].
Another view of texts might be that the books themselves contain information about
the relationships and interactions between individuals within a culture or multiple
cultures at the time the work was produced. On one level, they represent information
that may be fact or fiction. On another they contain information that has meaning in
a cultural context and “reproduce” in the histories of cultural systems. In this
respect, Dawkins’ concept of cultural
memes as a counterpart to genes in
evolutionary biology might be used to suggest that a book possesses a meme or its
content a number of memes that are somehow translated to whomever interacts with the
book. However, unlike genes, memes undergo continuous mutations [
Dawkins 1978, 208–209] because each individual experience differs,
making the fidelity of a given meme ambiguous in terms of the process of culture. In
other words, the interpretation of the information in the book constantly changes
through individual experience. Notwithstanding, this Darwinian analogy of cultural
evolution has found favor with some researchers because it structures the idea of
culture as an evolutionary process such that new techniques might be used to explore
the process
[4]. However, as researchers at the
London School of Economics point out, memes have no constant physical representation
making them
per se difficult to substantiate and study [
Howlett 2011].
Fortunately or not, when studying culture, we are left with cultural products such as
books and their ambiguous content as sources of information on the past, making
research difficult. For our study, we suggest that the ambiguity of interpretations
arising out of historical texts should not eliminate them as a viable source for
historical research into culture. In fact, we take the view that individuals interact
with each other through a process of encounter and self-identification, and that
through a number of different processes, these encounters impact on individuals
leading to a group-level dynamic [
Sperber 2007]. The key point is that
we are not seeking to identify a specific idea or meme and follow its evolution over
time. Rather we focus on individual behaviors based on what we believe to be an
original point of understanding based on a text. From here, our interest is, as
Daniel Sperber writes, how “Cultural information spreads across
members of a population through their interactions, that is, through their
producing, in their common environment, events and objects that carry
information that others can pick up”
[
Sperber 2007].
[5] The cultural information we are interested in relates to
self-identification.
As noted above, our research interest is the Hispanic, more specifically the
Southwestern U.S. and, to some extent, Northern Mexico. These are modern-labels
placed on a landscape previously unencumbered by political divisions such as an
international border between Mexico and the United States, and subdivisions such as
California, Arizona, New Mexico and Texas, or Baja California, Sonora, Chihuahua,
Coahuila Nuevo Leon, Tamaulipas. In fact, as Arias and Meléndez note, this mapping of
the region begins with the exploration and conquest of these lands by numerous
Europeans and their attempt to contextualize their own experiences [
Arias and Meléndez 2002]. This being the case we return to first contact between
Europeans and the pre-existing indigenous groups in a specific area of the
Mexico-U.S. Borderlands around present-day Santa Fe, New Mexico, because this area
represents a region known to have been inhabited by Pueblo Indians prior to first
contact and subsequently inhabited by the Spanish and later Hispanics. It is an area
of continuous settlement that serves as a starting place for our research on the
emergence of the Hispanic. We will use modern-day labels for consistency.
Four Spanish Borderlands texts, or
crónicas, were studied for
their clues as to the possible decisions that Spaniards employed when encountering
the Pueblo Indians of New Mexico. It is important to note that the Spanish writings
were produced under very strict regulations governing these official reports to the
Spanish monarchs from the New World. In fact, Valcárcel (2010) and Mignolo (1999,
1995) maintain the
relación (account), typical of writings between
1505 and 1573,
[6] operated under two sets of rules, the ones handed down by the
Crown on the requirements for these reports and those issued by the local
governors.
[7] Since
Native groups were an integral part of the process of first contact, where possible
we attempted to find the Native American voice in these stories. In the Pueblo case,
the vast majority of the written record stems from research and interests external to
their world; that is, by non-Pueblo authors such as the early chroniclers working
under strict guidelines. This presented a significant challenge to any attempt to
evaluate the Native experience of first contact, given that so much has been written
and documented by “outsiders.” Notwithstanding, one means of gauging the Pueblo
view of their history was through their testimonies as contained in the
crónicas. The Spanish colonial records submitted by secular and
religious groups recorded Native American belief systems, customs, social
organization and economic activities but to find these same materials written by
Native Americans themselves is another matter. Nevertheless, embedded within the
crónicas are fleeting glimpses of these groups. Any early
Native view contained in the crónicas was mediated through the Spanish writer. We
have also consulted a second source: works by modern-day members of the Pueblo, some
of whom have endeavored to record and preserve their histories in writing.
Two chronicles consulted deal with Francisco de Coronado’s (1540) expedition to
modern day New Mexico and Kansas in search of Cíbolo and Quiviria. Captain Juan
Jaramillo’s chronicle was written some 20 years after the expedition returned from
its travels and is considered by historians as a useful primary source on the
geography of the region [
Jaramillo 2004].
[8]
Jaramillo’s text makes references to encounters with indigenous groups in terms of
marching past “
rancherias
” or staying near “indiezuelos” and “ranchos,” where Indians cultivated
beans, maize, and squash (“Fue ansí, y
todo lo que por allí vimos, fue unos indiezuelos en algunos valles poblados, como
en ranchería, tierra estéril…”) [
Jaramillo 2004]. Although
fleeting in their representations, these references portray contact with indigenous
groups that transpired without incident (“...llegamos a otro arroyo a donde estaban
unos indios poblados que tenían ranchos de paja y sementeras de maíz y frisoles y
calabazas…”). Jaramillo’s account also includes descriptions of tense even
hostile encounters between the explorers and tribes in what is now the Great Plains
of the U.S. (“Aquí donde hallamos los indios y nos vieron, se comenzaron de
alborotar con voces y muestras de huir, y aun tenían allí algunos sus mujeres
consigo, …”). There is evidence of cooperation as we noted the importance of
translators or collaborators with the Spanish (“comenzoles a llamar el indio
Isopete en su lengua,…”). In fact, Adolphus Bandelier emphasized that the
interpreters who travelled with the Spanish explorers were almost certainly from
different indigenous groups, such as the Mexica or Tlaxcanas of Central Mexico — two
of the many groups that accompanied the Spanish — were not always at hand when
encounters occurred, and when they were, there was a question of competency as well
as fidelity in the work of translation. Between Coronado’s
entrada
and the
reconquest, modern surveys of the area suggest that very
little Spanish entered into the native languages. That is, the Pueblo retained
linguistic purity [
Bandelier 1910, 3].
The second chronicle is Pedro de Castañeda’s narrative of the same Coronado
expedition [
Winship 2009]. Just as Jaramillo wrote some 20 years after
the fact, Castañeda also recalls events two decades later, but his motivation for
writing the chronicle is to correct the misleading accounts of the mythical land of
Cíbolo. He wrote that the furthest reaches of the New Spain did not hold great wealth
and populations like those in Mexico or Peru. His report tells us that in some
instances the expedition encountered depopulated areas or relatively small
settlements ranging from several hundred to perhaps one thousand inhabitants. This is
significant because modern scholarship suggests that the population of the region was
on the order of 50,000–60,000 at the time of first contact with the Spanish [
Flint and Flint 2012]. There is some continuity between Castañeda and Jaramillo,
as both write that native populations are seen to live in
rancherias and sustain themselves from hunting and other
activities. Castañeda’s narrative contains more detail on the landscape and distance
between the indigenous communities and larger topographical features. As he explains,
Cíbola was not unpopulated; indigenous populations lived in communities separated by
several
leguas but in the eyes of Spanish conquistadores were
considered a single agglomeration of population. Modern-day researchers find the
Castañeda account particularly important in terms human geography.
The third Spanish chronicle is by Gaspar Castaño de Sosa,
[9]
alcalde mayor of modern-day Monterrey, Nuevo León (Villa de San
Luís), during the late sixteenth century. When the original governor of the region,
Luis de Carvajal, was denounced as a crypto-Jew, Castaño de Sosa was elevated to
teniente de gobernador of Almadén (present day Monclova, Nuevo
León).
[10] In 1590 Castaño de Sosa was forced to leave
Almadén having been himself denounced before the Real Audencía by Juan Morlete as a
slave trader and possible crypto-Jew. His flight from Monterrey to Santa Fe was
published in 1592, and again in a collection of writings from the Archivo de Indias
in 1871. Gaspar Castaño de Sosa’s
Memoria is an
interesting document to the extent that it details the exploits and encounters of
this unauthorized expedition to New Mexico some eight years ahead of Juan de Oñate,
whose conquest would lead to a more permanent settlement of New Mexico [
Castaño de Sosa 2004]. The work itself has received relatively little critical
attention when compared to the more known exploits such as those of Coronado or Juan
de Oñate.
[11] While the historical significance of
this chronicle is not the focus of this research
per se, it is
interesting that Castaño de Sosa names and takes possession of lands as he makes his
way to Santa Fe, following the norms established by the Spanish Crown. We read in his
narrative that the first encounter with the indigenous populations, described as
gente de paz (literally “peaceful people”), is facilitated
by a native identified as Miguel, who is familiar with Spanish, having served with an
unidentified group of soldiers from whom he learned the language. He is welcomed into
the Castaño de Sosa retinue as a translator [
Castaño de Sosa 2004]. Another
interesting aspect of Castaño de Sosa’s
Memoria is that
encounters with Native Americans are almost a daily occurrence and seemingly friendly
by all accounts [
Castaño de Sosa 2004]. However, it is curious that in this
time period, we find Indians who have already acquiesced to the will of the Spanish
Monarchy and the Catholic Church. In the first one hundred years after Cortés, the
north-western frontier was, according to Castañeda, at Culiacán in Sinaloa [
Winship 2009]. The northern frontier according to Matson and Schroeder
was at the San Gregorio River in southern Chihuahua in 1581, although slave raids had
reached beyond [
Schroeder and Matson 1965 , 6]. In fact, Capitan Lope de
Aritit had reached the Concho River in search of slaves. Captain Mateo Gonzales had
taken slaves from the junction of the Rio Grande and Rio Conchos, and Gaspar de Luxan
also entered the region in 1581. The north-eastern frontier during this period would
have been at Monterrey (present-day Mexico) at best. The fact that slaving raids were
taking place indicates that this region was beyond Spanish control, which means it
remained virtually impossible that as Castaño de Sosa moved across this region, he
truly found native groups who had submitted to Spanish authority. Notwithstanding,
the Castaño de Sosa report contains many references to encounters with indigenous
groups that were mostly friendly but turned hostile once he reached modern-day Santa
Fe [
Castaño de Sosa 2004].
The last Spanish chronicle consulted discusses the travels of two Franciscan friars,
Francisco Domínguez and Silvestre Vélez de Escalante, who set out from Santa Fe, New
Mexico in 1776 to open a route to Monterey, California. Thanks to Father Escalante’s
chronicling of the expedition, their attempt to link New Mexico with the Pacific
coast could be considered a final point in the cycle of Spanish conquest and
exploration of North America. Many U.S.-Mexico Borderlands scholars view the content
of this record as an important contribution to knowledge on the Pueblo Indians and
Hispanic life in the present-day U.S. Southwest. The Domínguez-Escalante
Derrotero y
diario (route and diary)
consists of documents not the least important of which is Father Escalante’s report
(Vélez de Escalante and Domínguez 1777).
[12] Among the supporting
documents in the archive is a 16-page analysis by Juan Augustín Morfí of the contents
of the
Derrotero y
diario and its general relevance to the entire Spanish enterprise
of conquest and discovery. Because Morfí’s analysis represents a consolidation of
views held by authorities and scholars of the late-eighteenth century, it is
revealing in terms of Spanish perceptions of European-Native encounters and views of
a continuously ambiguous frontier region. Morfí is also known in the Borderlands for
his work on the history of Texas, completed as part of the Teodore de Croix
inspection (1777).
[13] The contents of Morfí’s comments are
significant because they reveal a number of considerations. Firstly, as late as the
end of the eighteenth century, communication between New Mexico and “old” Mexico
was limited meaning the colonial seat at Santa Fe functioned in relative isolation.
Morfi also writes that of all the indigenous groups in America (North, Central and
South), the only ones to have actually declared “war” with the Spanish are the
Apaches and Comanches in the north. This is significant because, if true, the fact
highlights a great difference between the northern and other frontier-regions in
Spanish America. Finally, his writings highlight that Native Americans were far from
monolithic and their inter-relationships were as much a balancing act between
themselves and their distinct cultures as their experiences with the Spanish. Morfi’s
work also gives us a more concrete view of how the Spanish crown thought to delimit
its boundaries, following geographic landmarks such as the Colorado River and the Red
River. The urgency to label, define and delimit was sparked by French incursions in
Texas and Louisiana, and Russian and British explorers in the Upper Pacific
Northwest. Father Morfí’s analysis of the Domínguez-Escalante diaries serves as an
excellent bookend to the reading of these chronicles because it truly represents an
end to the cycles of conquest and discovery. Subsequent writers would increasingly
address military and political considerations rather than the raw nature of the
region, taking for granted that Spain had imprinted itself on the land and its
people.
In terms of what might have been said or written from the perspective of Native
America during this early period, precious little survives. One source may be found
in the documents of the Juan de Oñate expedition which ended with a pogrom against
the Ácoma Pueblo in 1599. The details of this event have been well researched, most
notably by historians such Marc Simmons (1993) as well as H.H. Bancroft (1889), G.
Hammond and A. Rey (1953). An official enquiry into the reports of atrocities
committed during the pogrom reached Mexico and Spain, leading to the trials,
subsequent conviction, and banishment of Oñate from New Mexico. These records include
statements from numerous eyewitnesses including natives of central Mexico who
traveled with Oñate and a number of Ácoma.
[14] For example, a native from Tlatenango in the present-day state
of Puebla is reported to have companied Oñate to New Mexcio.
In the wake of the Pueblo Revolt, the Spanish, along with 317 Tiwas, retreated to
Paso del Norte, leading to the establishment of Nuestra Señora de Corpus Christie
mission and the first significant populating of present day C. Juárez, Chihuahua — El
Paso, Texas.
[15] John
Kessell (1989) writes that the news of the Pueblo success against the Spanish
encouraged other groups to test their possibilities. Although never brought to
fruition, the rumblings of a revolt at Paso del Norte precipitated an enquiry into
the plotting of a rebellion, which is another source of the Native voice.
[16] The significance of this enquiry is the fact that
whilst the Northern Pueblo were enjoying a long decade of cultural renaissance, the
Southern Pueblo remained under Spanish control, albeit by choice given that they
retreated with them to Paso del Norte. Still, this fact did not constitute a complete
acquiesce to Spanish rule. In segments of testimony from the trails we read that the
governor of the Tigua and his lieutenant, as well as representatives from the Piros,
were called before a tribunal presided over by Domingo Jirona de Cruzate, governor
and captain general of the Spanish at Yselta. The testimony begins by stating that
the governor of the Tigua, his lieutenant, and two Piro Indians were brought before
the tribunal to give testimony. We also learn that Manos Indians, already
Christianized, are also in the area and friendly with the two other groups. One of
the Piros, Ventura, understands the Manso language, thereby allowing the Piros and
Tiguas to communicate about a potential uprising. In another interesting and very
relevant segment in the testimony, we read of the inter-marriages. The point here,
without reading too much into the primary texts, is that at every moment some form of
individual cultural contact was taking place, whether this occurred intra- or
extra-tribally, is to some extent unimportant. However, we can see that Pueblo, Piro,
Manso, Apache, and others interacted with each other as well as with the Spanish.
The work of Joe Sando,
[17] a native of the present-day Jemez Pueblo,
[18] explains that considerable differences
existed between and within native groups. He maintains that the Pueblo underwent
fewer fundamental changes than other tribes who were subjected to forced relocation
and even complete destruction. This perseverance is attributed by Sando to an
attitude of “accommodation” toward European cultures. Of course, this notion of
“accommodation” is of great interest to this research because it suggests
that beyond individual encounters or interactions, a broader strategy existed within
the Pueblo that permitted the emergence of a cultural cohesiveness.
[19] Another highly regarded Pueblo scholar, Alfonso
Alex Ortiz, a member of the modern-day San Juan Pueblo, writing in 1994, frames the
question of Pueblo culture in a more fundamental way: He asks if it makes sense to
lump the Pueblos together in one group [
Ortiz 1994, 296].
[20]
“Does the term Pueblo, or Indian for that
matter, truly represent the relative or is it an artificial construct developed by
the Spaniards and perpetuated by later historians, anthropologists and the
like?” This, Ortiz maintains, is the fundamental question at the heart of
whether or not a “Pueblo” culture survives. That is, do the “Pueblo”
themselves know if they are different from other groups they know or have known?
Ortiz writes that he believes it can be demonstrated that the Pueblo people have
believed themselves to share a common culture despite linguistic differences. In
other words, their cultural cohesiveness is not based on language as is often
considered key to this notion. Ortiz offers that there is no single institution that
unifies the groups over the past 2,000 years [
Ortiz 1994, 297].
However, a non-social structural and non-cultural factor is shared by all surviving
Pueblos, and as Sando also noted, the Pueblo have never been displaced from their
homeland. It is the sense of place that predominates and unifies the Pueblos [
Ortiz 1994, 297]. He writes that the Pueblo had a long history of
contact well before the arrival of the Spanish, much of which is only subject to
conjecture in modern day research [
Ortiz 1994, 298]. There is a
broad history of cross-cultural contact between the groups that encompasses material
trade in food, wares, and clothing, religious contact, and repeated contact such that
the Pueblo tribes have, “touched each
other’s lives in the most fundamental of ways”
[
Ortiz 1994, 299]. Furthermore, the network of contacts extended
into Meso-America. The experience with Coronado later served as a learning tool for
future generations of Pueblos. Specifically, Ortiz writes that taking refuge in the
ancestral mountains was known to happen on many occasions during the Spanish period
and helped the groups survive [
Ortiz 1994, 300]. He offers as
examples the fact that the Tewa fled to the Hopi and the Jemez to the Navajo.
With the writings of Alfonso Ortiz and Joe Sando, this research can formulate a more
general picture of the Pueblo view of first encounters and ongoing contacts. Clearly,
there were misunderstandings in terms of signals and their meanings despite the best
effort of the Spanish to use non-Pueblo translators to infer and imply significance.
It is clear that the pattern of individual and group contact resulted in exchanges
that in turn resulted in cultural modifications, the extent of which is contentious
at best. Nevertheless, we see that there is an element of ambivalence in the Pueblo
view of their contact with the Spanish. For example, Ortiz in particular expresses
animosity toward the European intrusion and its consequences for Pueblo life;
however, he also notes that accommodation was instrumental to their resilience. Sando
appears less reticent in his observations on the Spanish impact on natives, in
general. Both agree that Native groups interacted with each other on a number of
levels and that the Spanish and other European groups were not the only cultural
factors influencing their way of life. It is interesting to note that neither author
underscores the presence of Tlacaltecans, Méxica, nor other groups from the south as
particularly influential; rather they are clear that regional contacts with each
other and tribes from the Great Plains were more significant.
We believe we have gleaned data from these texts on individual choices about
self-identification as well as cultural group formation. We take the view that the
literary record contains information about individual decision-making and that
aggregations of these decisions are an appropriate analogy for cultural beginnings,
transformations, and advancements that can be used to explore how behaviors lead to
more complex systems such as culture. In this regard, our reading of the text has
given us an idea of the individuals who participated in the cultural system, and the
types of rules that governed their behaviors.
Agent-Based Modeling for the Humanities
The process of self-identification contained in the chronicles is based on rules
governing individual action within a context and includes a number of aspects, among
them decision-making. We focus our efforts on decision-making because it is
particularly well-suited to computer coding easily translatable to mathematics. Table
1 presents examples of text-based references identified as behaviors manifested by
Spanish and Pueblo individuals upon contact. These references were translated into a
format more appropriate for game theory (GT), which is nothing more than a
mathematical expression of different people interacting with each other, sometimes
cooperatively, and other times in competition with each other, but certainly
expressing both potential behaviors to varying degrees. John Holland notes, games,
strategies, payoffs and other scientific metaphors are characterized by rules that
are more readily converted to “mathematical” analysis [
Holland 1998]. He also stresses that this process is not absolute, in and of itself; however,
the decisions of the two individuals is important within the context of other
simultaneous decision-makers such that a blending of players’ mutual and conflicting
interests makes the combination of multi-agent simulation and GT interesting for this
research. While this world of games and their mathematical expression exhibits some
explanatory powers, the important point is that a wide variety of outcomes may be
considered. Furthermore, the human behaviors modeled take place outside a market
construct: In other words, rather than using a market, pricing model, and utility
theory, game theory centers on strategies and choices.
Source
|
Interpretation
|
Choice/Strategy
|
Juan Jaramillo’s Relación
|
|
|
“
..un camino todo poblado y en paz…
”
|
Already intermixing |
Cooperate always |
Pedro de Castañeda
|
|
|
“
…diciendo que un hombre le había forzado a su mujer…
”
|
Violent encounter |
Defect |
Gaspar Castaño de Sosa
|
|
|
“
…y dos días antes, vino a la dicha villa, un indio, llamado
Miguel…
”
|
Friendly encounter |
Cooperate |
Analysis of Juan Morfí
|
|
|
“
Los Tarahumaras y algunos otros cuando quieren reducir el de la
obediencia hallan la misma protección en los Navajos y Lipánes.
”
|
Inter-tribal cooperation |
Cooperate |
Ácoma trials
|
|
|
Al ser preguntado por qué este confesante y los demás indios de su
pueblo mataron al dicho [maestro] de campo y a otros diez españoles y a dos
[chicos], dijo que los dichos españoles hirieron a un indio del pueblo y que
por esto se enojaron y los mataron.
|
Violent encounter |
Defect |
Joe Sando
|
|
|
“…an attitude of ‘accommodation’ toward
European cultures…”
|
|
Cooperate |
Alfonso Ortiz
|
|
|
“…the Pueblo tribes have, ‘touched each
other’s lives in the most fundamental of ways.’
”
|
Tolerance for ideas |
Cooperate |
Ysleta del Sur
|
|
|
“
…pues los dichos Pedro y Ventura eran de nación Piros y los indios
todos eran uno, y eran sus amigos los Tiguas del pueblo de la Isleta, les
fueron a hablar y a convocarlos para que todos juntos
ejecutasen…
”
|
Inter-tribal competition |
Cooperate |
Table 1.
Table 1 - Select Text Analysis, Encounter, and Choice/Strategy
[21]
Instead of counting the number of passages in which an encounter between Spanish and
Native American was mentioned, we translated passages into game theory and computer
code for multi-agent simulation. Table 1 also presents a short list of
interpretations and translations of behaviors for encoding. The artificial world of
ABMs allows us to imagine a virtual space in which individuals have a limited number
of choices, fulfilling one of the fundamental criteria of complexity — that a limited
set of simple rules govern individual behavior [
Aschenwald 2002].
The cellular automata model is a common ABM because it incorporates numerous
individuals.
[22] Each cell represents an individual — referred to as agents
— that resides on a grid in a certain state, chosen from a small number of clearly
defined possible states, such as “on”/“off” or “alive”/“dead.”
The visualization (see Figures 1 and 2) is similar to a sheet of
graph paper of arbitrary size, dimension, and geometry with a certain (randomized)
initial setting. In a dynamic model, the future state of each agent is determined by
its current state as well as the current states of its neighbors. This is important
in complexity because the context of each individual influences his or her state. In
Figure 1 we see one yellow cell surrounded by eight blue cells. This is understood as
an individual surrounded by eight other individuals. In the simulations, the yellow
cell selects one of its eight neighbors and communicates information based on a set
of rules that are uniform for all cells [
Cochinos 2000, 41].
[23] The outcome of the information
exchange is conditioned on the status of one or more of its neighbors.
[24] In this way, the model is set such that the rules
governing an individual’s behavior may be simple, but the outcomes of each encounter
by pairs of individuals when aggregated may become very complex, as shown in Figure
2.
Game theory figures into the simulations when individual actions are expressed in
terms of choices based on a decision strategy. A number of researchers ([
Page and Bednar 2007], [
Boyd and Richerson 2005], [
Axelrod and Hammond 2006])
have sought to understand phenomena such as culture through the lens of complexity .
In these and other works some form of game theory and complexity have been combined.
We are following their research models by opting to use decision-making strategies as
proxies for cultural attitude, as these lend themselves to encoding. This means
individuals of a particular culture possess a common attitude toward members of their
own as well as toward other cultures. The result of each encounter between
individuals can subsequently be expressed as a payout (cost or benefit). For example,
two individuals —
A1 and
A — encounter each
other. As they are pre-programmed with a behavior because of their culture, they
identify the other based on a notion of self and make a decision to interact or not,
as the case may be. This interaction is termed cooperate (C) or not cooperate (D). In
code, this cooperate situation would be expressed as CC if both individuals
cooperate. When one of the two differs, CD and DC are the possible combinations. In
the case of both refusing to cooperate with each other, this produces DD. Table 2
lists the possible behaviors of each individual, and Table 3 translates these into
coded outcomes.
Table 2
|
Types of Behaviors Possible |
Always Cooperate |
Individual always cooperates |
Alternate |
Individual alternates behavior no matter what experience |
Tit-for-tat |
Individual alternates depending on previous experience |
Grudger |
Individual begins with cooperate and defects |
Always Defect |
Defect agent always changes strategy |
Table 3
|
Coded Outcomes |
CC |
Both Cooperate A1 and A2 |
CD |
Agent A1 cooperates, Agent A2 does not |
DC |
Agent A2 cooperates, Agent A1 does not |
DD |
Agents never cooperate |
What does each individual gain from each encounter? In our view, interactions between
individuals do not simply end with a code. We have taken the position that
individuals are impacted by contact since it involves an exchange of information.
This requires additional thought on possible outcomes. As noted above, other
researchers have used biological notions such as reproductive capabilities or life
spans as features of social systems. This, we thought, would help incorporate a
dynamic population that might approximate the Borderlands. Consequently, when the
encounters resulted in an outcome of C, the ability to reproduce of an individual
improves. On the other hand when the encounter generates a D, the individual and
consequently the population experiences a decline in reproductive opportunities;
stated another way: the possibility of death increases for any individual in the
entire population.
More importantly, as we are interested in showing that encounters between individuals
of different cultures can impact on individual perceptions of self, we translated
this into “tolerance,” here defined based on how each individual views his or
her neighbor in terms of self. The idea is that a member of a cultural group has a
predefined notion of self, but this may change as members of other cultures are
encountered or even as members of own-culture who have experienced outsiders are
encountered. In this way, we have attempted to incorporate the notion of self as one
of perception and the question of cultural group membership as a question of scale or
granularity from the perspective of the observer. This means that on one level there
are the inter- and intra-cultural interactions, represented by individual agents and
their interactions with each other. On another level, there is the cultural whole
operating within a larger system of multiple cultures. The vision from above is one
in which large cultural complexes can be seen as different, but from below, there is
individual as well as subgroup-level diversity. As a general rule, we have taken the
position that an individual’s tolerance for difference improves with positive
encounters (C) with members of a different culture group. Conversely, an in encounter
that produces a negative experience (D), the individual with D becomes more
intolerant. The degree to which these changes take place are predetermined by the
researcher. Referring back to Figure 1, we see that each individual cell has eight
neighbors, and each of the other neighbors has eight neighbors. In other words, each
pair of individuals is part of a larger system of pairs of individuals who are
simultaneously making decisions based on their culture. However, because the process
of decision-making is iterative and takes place simultaneously over multitudes of
pairs of individuals, notions of self can change, and when they do, the summation of
these individual interactions generates a group-level dynamic from which additional
cultural groups emerge.
We tested our ideas on group-level diversity using an ABM titled Borderlands5 in
which the landscape is represented as a two-dimensional finite grid with each cell
“alive” (color) or “dead” (black).
[25]
Individuals may be alone or belong to groups (differentiated by color) meant to
simulate the sharing of common characteristics. An important feature of our model is
the fact that individuals are immobile, that is they do not move from the cell to
which they have been assigned, but they will interact with one of the individuals
occupying a neighboring cell (out of eight possible) giving the illusion of movement.
Each original individual is born into the landscape with a culture that is
represented by one color (vector). Each color corresponds to a decision-making
strategy to be used when encountering an individual or culture. All individuals of
the world work simultaneously over time, measured in terms of ticks of the ABM clock.
Additionally, all individuals follow the rules of the world in a synchronous manner
and modify their individual parameters accordingly. The process is as follows:
- At any given step in time (or tick of the clock), a cell will randomly select
one of its neighbors, forming a pair.
- The two individuals decide to cooperate or not based on a pre-determined
approach (culture group membership).
- Depending on the outcome of the decision because another individual is
involved, a number of additional variables adjust in order to capture a
population-wide impact or outcome.
Our actual focus is self-identification, consequently the agent does not move rather
she perceives what is happening around her. In our experiment, self-identification is
not static rather it is subject to change based on a change in “tolerance.” In
Borderlands5 this factor is designed to approximate the process of
self-identification based on cultural similarity. Tolerance is expressed as a value
between zero (0) and (1) and is set by the researcher such that it may be very
strictly defined or very flexible. In the best-case scenario, if both sides
cooperate, both sides become more tolerant. In the worst-case scenario, neither is
tolerant, and no one cooperates. In the two intermediate positions (A1 cooperates but
A2 does not, vice versa) either Individual A1 or A2 will receive a
positive benefit from cooperative behavior despite the fact that the other is
uncooperative. At the beginning of the simulation, all individuals have a tolerance
level of zero (0), although this changes as the model progresses. Zero tolerance
means that there is no opportunity for cooperation based on similarity — agents must
be exactly alike. When the tolerance level is increased (tolerance > 0.0)
cooperative behavior extends beyond those agents who are exactly alike to everyone
who is the same. If the tolerance level is pushed to the other extreme (0), everyone
is intolerant of all other agents and chaos ensues; each individual sees herself as
unique. As long as individuals A1 and A2 determine that the color of the other is
less than the pre-set tolerance factor, each individual recognizes the other as from
the “same” culture and implements a strategy that corresponds to interactions
with somebody of the “same” culture. On the other hand, if the distance is
greater than stipulated by the system, an alternative strategy will be played. It is
important to note that if one of the individuals is more tolerant than the other, it
can transpire that one individual “interprets” that both are of the same
culture, while the other “interprets” that the cultures are different. This is
ambiguous yes, but it is this unexplained phenomenon that permits individuals to
possess a more fluid self-identity within limits. By setting up the model as
described, we are conforming to the guidelines of complexity, that a limited number
of rules govern individual action and impact on a small number of parameters. And
yet, there is a broader context that is capture by population-wide factors.
In Borderlands5 we created a world with three cultures each occupying a different
location in the space; however, the total population does not exceed 50 percent of
available cells. We have deliberately created several “borders” between groups
(see Figures 3 and 4). These are meant to simulate
distinct separations between groups such as physical barriers — rivers or mountains.
What does this all mean? In our abstraction, we are visualizing three cultural groups
one of whom is Spanish and the other two distinct Native American groups — one larger
than the other. These cultures possess cultural attributes expressed as strategies.
The largest of the Native American groups will always alternate strategies as they
encounter members of the two smaller groups. One of the smaller groups will always
operate with the largest group (say a Spanish behavior), and the other will only use
an alternating strategy with the smaller group (say Native-Spanish ambivalence). Each
member of a cultural group always cooperates with those identified as from the same
culture. This is one of our interpretations of how first and ongoing contact might
have taken place between the Spanish and Native Americans.
Since our interest is the group-level experience, we grouped the individual cells in
Figure 4 along color lines. Our baseline was the initial setting as seen in Figure 5.
The images that follow (Figures 7 through 9) show how the original three groups have
evolved over time, giving rise to additional groups — that a far more complex system
of identities. The passage of time was simulated by allowing our model to run for a
period of 6,000 ticks of the ABM clock (in our imagination something akin to 500
years). The initial settings were not altered except in one specific way — density of
population. This means that in a series of experiments, we altered the number of
active cells along the following lines 25%, 50%, 75%, and 100%. Our intent was to
explore in greater detail the role of population density in group formation. These
variations were run 20 times in each case and results were essentially the same: an
average of 10–11 new groups emerged from the initial settings governing individual
actions. This is interesting to the extent that it suggests that population density
may not be a determining factor in cultural group formation, rather the key component
may be individual cooperative or uncooperative behavior. Whether or not this outcome
reflects reality requires additional testing by us as well as researchers who might
be interested in this methodology and process. Notwithstanding, the fact that our
model does not actually incorporate geography possible thorough the use of digitized
topography maps, does not invalidate our findings. Rather, we would like to suggest
that self-identification contributes to group formation not only on land but may also
be the case in cyberspace. In which case, our research only leads to more questions
and applications.
Conclusions
We have used literary studies as a broad guideline for translating historical texts
into an agent-based modeling experiment on a small aspect of culture — how it might
evolve over time into a diverse and complex system. The individual actors we have
read about in the accounts of Spanish conquistadors and Native Americans have been
transformed into computer-based agents operating in a virtual world. Their individual
attributes have become colors, and each interacts using a game in which their
decisions center on cooperation; that is, the choices are presented as a form of game
theory in which any one individual may defect or cooperate with the outcome impacting
on the tolerance of other cultures as well as their ability to reproduce. In other
words, we have created an interdisciplinary view of culture: one that involves
humanities, complexity and computational technology.
We have used a great deal of imagination and creativity in our research — virtual
studies require this of the researcher. And although there are limitations, we can
see that culture may be visualized and studied in this new environment. That is to
say, a humanistic background does not preclude researchers from attempting to use the
latest technology to explore ideas common to the humanities. However, it does require
that we consider how our ideas translate into methods outside our disciplinary
boundaries. In this case we have used complexity science in conjunction with text
analysis as methodological approach for studying the intricacies of human
interactions. This is a bottom-up approach that focuses on the interactions of
individuals and their role in generating larger and more complex structures that
cannot be predicted in a linear fashion from the characteristics of the individual
actors. Furthermore, we have shown how a small set of simple ideas can lead to a
broader outcome. This is at the very heart of complexity and agent-based modeling.
Our research suggests that this methodology introduces some interesting benefits for
the study of culture and its emergence and transformation over time; however, much of
this exploration is taking place outside the humanities.
Appendix 1
Source
|
Interpretation
|
Choice/Strategy
|
Juan Jaramillo’s Relación
|
|
|
“
..un camino todo poblado y en paz…
”
|
Already intermixing |
Cooperate always |
“
… aquí vimos un indio y dos que parecieron ser después de la
primera población…
”
|
Sighting |
|
“
... todos estos indios….nos recibieron bien
”
|
Already intermixing |
Cooperate always |
Pedro de Castañeda
|
|
|
..parecía en otro tiempo haber sido casa fuerte en tiempo que fue poblada y
bien se conocía ser hecha por gentes extranjeras políticas y guerreras venidas
de lejos … |
Inter-tribal warfare/politics |
Grudger |
“
…en lugar de poblados, hallar grandes despoblados, y en lugar de
ciudades populosas, hallar pueblos de doscientos vecinos y el mayor de
ochocientos o mil;
”
|
groupings of natives |
group vector |
“
…diciendo que un hombre le había forzado a su mujer…
”
|
Violent encounter |
Grudger |
“
…hubo lugar que las lenguas hablasen con ellos y se les hiciese
requerimientos por ser gente bien entendida…
”
|
Linguistic contact - interpreter |
Cooperative |
“
…otro día fue Don Garcia López de Cárdenas a ver los pueblos y
tomar de ellos lengua
”
|
Linguistic contact - interpreter |
Cooperative |
Gaspar Castaño de Sosa
|
|
|
“
…y dos días antes, vino a la dicha villa, un indio, llamado
Miguel…
”
|
Friendly encounter |
Cooperate |
“
… y que dio, acabo de tres días, con muy gran cantidad de gente de
nación despeguen, el cual fue de ellos muy bien recibido…
”
|
Grouping of natives, friendly |
Cooperate |
“
…no dio lengua ninguna, de muchas que llevaba, que le entendiesen
persona…
”
|
Linguistic contact - Interpreter |
Cooperate |
“
…quedó el dicho Joan de Vega, indio; y visto que quedaba solo,
asieron del algunos indios, y lo echaron en el río y le quitaron unas
amarras, y le dieron tres flechazos…
”
|
Violent encounter - Natives |
Defect |
Analysis of Juan Morfí
|
|
|
“
Los Tarahumaras y algunos otros cuando quieren reducir el de la
obediencia hallan la misma protección en los Navajos y Lipánes.
”
|
Inter-tribal cooperation |
Cooperate |
“
Y no es increíble que algunos Comanches, de los que se califica más
fieles, se empeñasen, por algún premio, en acompañar a la expedición, en
calidad de guías, mientras sus Parientes se ocupan en la
guerra.
”
|
Tribe joins Spanish |
Defect |
Ácoma trials
|
|
|
“
En este dicho día luego incontinente el dicho señor gobernador hizo
[presentarse] ante si a un indio que mediante el dicho intérprete dijo
llamarse Caucachi.
”
|
Linguistic contact - interpreter |
Cooperate |
Al ser preguntado por qué este confesante y los demás indios de su
pueblo mataron al dicho [maestro] de campo y a otros diez españoles y a dos
[chicos], dijo que los dichos españoles hirieron a un indio del pueblo y que
por esto se enojaron y los mataron.
|
Violent encounter |
Defect |
Joe Sando
|
|
|
“Thus, the Pueblos have common elements, but
are distinctive entities in their own right”
|
Cultural Unity/Differentiation |
Tit-for-tat |
“…an attitude of ‘accommodation’ toward
European cultures…”
|
|
Cooperate |
“…the writings of Bartolomé de las Casas and
Francisco de Vitoria, were responsible for a change in Spanish attitudes
after the re-conquest of New Mexico in the 1690s. He suggests that their
writings lead to a policy of mutual accommodation…”
|
Tolerance or accomodation |
Cooperate |
Alfonso Ortiz
|
|
|
“Ortiz writes that he believes it can be
demonstrated that the Pueblo people have believed themselves to share a
common culture despite linguistic differences.”
|
Cultural Unity/Differentiation |
Tit-for-tat |
“…the Pueblo tribes have, ‘touched each
other’s lives in the most fundamental of ways.’
”
|
Tolerance for ideas |
Cooperate |
“…the Pueblo tribes have, ‘touched each
other’s lives in the most fundamental of ways.’
”
|
Tolerance for ideas |
Cooperate |
Ysleta del Sur
|
|
|
“
…pues los dichos Pedro y Ventura eran de nación Piros y los indios
todos eran uno, y eran sus amigos los Tiguas del pueblo de la Isleta, les
fueron a hablar y a convocarlos para que todos juntos
ejecutasen…
”
|
Inter-tribal competition |
Cooperate |
Table 4.
Table 1 - Extended Text Analysis, Encounter, and Choice/Strategy
Notes
[1] E.
Slingerland suggests the humanities have much to offer natural sciences as
discoveries possess new challenges to our understanding of us, each other, and
life [Slingerland 2008]. He also writes that the humanities can
benefit from science such that the two may be integrated. Human experiences are
part of human existence and must be studied as an integrated whole. Consequently,
bringing the two branches of knowledge together is a goal of this
interdisciplinary work. [2] Taken from MIT Encyclopedia of Cognitive
Sciences, ed. Robert A. Wilson and Frank Keil. Boston, MA, MIT Press,
2001, p. 120. This definition is attributed to the goals of cognitive
anthropology.
[3] Determining how an individual came to recognize the need or
the ability to dominate the external world is not the purpose of this work
per se. However, the idea that a group-level behavior such
as “culture” emerged from individual decisions, is the foundation. In order
to pursue this line of investigation, an interdisciplinary approach is needed.
Therefore, extensive use is made of concepts normally found in the natural,
social, and computational sciences, in order to study the emergence of a specific
culture — in this case a cultural system labeled Hispanic.
[4] Whilst for Dan Sperber and Nicolas Claidière, culture spreads like
an addiction through individuals, but the transmission mechanism of cultural
information is more akin to that of epidemiology (virus) instead of natural
selection [Sperber and Claidière 2006]. [5] He also notes that, “The state of a culture at a given
moment corresponds to the distribution of variants resulting from these
micro-events, and the evolution of culture is that of this
distribution”
[Sperber 2007]. [6] See [Insula 1999]. The entire volume contains
analyses and commentaries on the crónicas, relaciones ,and other early colonial
writings. [7] In fact, in several secondary works the compendium of documents
and detailed analysis may be found in [Vázquez Coronado 2004]. [8] Scant information is
available on Juan Jaramillo, but [Flint and Flint 2012] offer similar
observations to those found in [Thrapp 1991]=, as well as other
works such as [Winship 2009], Herbert Eugene Bolton, George P.
Hammond and Agapito Rey, as well as a relatively new entry in the Cervantes Virtual Portal de la Cultural Chicana. [9] See Schroeder and
Matson 1965. Schroeder and Matson include a complete English translation of the
Alonso de Leon chapter on Castaño de Soso. According to this version Castaño de
Sosa’s “talents” were greatly esteemed by Governor Gomez Perez de las Marinas
on the entrada to the Islanda of Moluccas [Schroeder and Matson 1965 , 9].
They write that Andres Perez de Verlanga’s first hand account is the only known
account of the expedition ([Alessio Robles 1978], [Chipman 1992], [Weddle 1985]). Luis Carvajal was
considered one of the first to enter Texas from Mexico. He was also born in
Portugal. He and his wife Guiomar de Ribera were Jews converted to Christianity.
His wife was the daughter of a royal slave factor and originally from Lisbon.
Archivo General de Indias, Archivo General de Indias de
Sevilla (Madrid, 1958) (Duaine 1971, Toro 1944, Weddle and1985). [11] See [Bancroft 1889 ] and Dorothy Hulls’ summary. Matson
and Schroeder suggest that the journalist may have been Andres Perez, although
they are not certain [Schroeder and Matson 1965 , 17]. Interestingly, they
note that two Indians who remained with the Pueblo after Castaño de Sosa’s case
was remanded into the custody of Spain’s representatives in 1593 also appear in
the record regarding Juan de Oñate’s activities in the region [Schroeder and Matson 1965 , 16]. [12] The copy utilized for this research was
found at the Newberry Library in Chicago, Illinois.
[13] Among the publications that deal with this tour include
Diario y derrotero, 1777-1781, Compendio del diario del viaje a las
Provincias Internas [de fray Juan Agustín Morfi]: Chihuahua, 26 abril
1778, and History of Texas, 1673-1779
[Morfí 1967]. For his diary and travel routes in digital format, see
[Morfí 1967]. [14] Many thanks to the Research Center
for Romance Studies, International and Area Studies, University of California,
Berkeley; specifically, the generous help of Jerry R. Craddock, Director, who
kindly offered electronic copies of the center’s translation work which form part
of the Cíbola Project. Quotes for this section were taken with permission from
Jerry R. Craddock and John H. R. Polt (2008), specifically, as noted in the
document, the paleographic transcription of the primary text, collation with the
second text, and translation into English. This work is based on Archivo General
de Indias, Patronato, legajo, 22, ramo 13, ff. 1036r-1085r (94r-143r); ff.
1086r-1131v.
[15] Tiwa is part of the Tanoan-Kiowa language grouping. The members of
this tribe that retreated with the Spanish adopted Ysleta as part of their name
rather than Isleta, which was the name of their original settlement near
modern-day Albuquerque, New Mexico. Their modern name is Tigua.
[16] The
documents associated with this case are entitled Año de 1685 Numero 4
Autos sobre los socorros que pide [e]lgouernador de la Nueva Mexico y otras
noticias tocant[es] a la subleuazion de los yndios bar[ba]-ros de aquella
prouinzia y la mudanza del Puesto del Pa[so] [d]el Rio del Norte al de la
Ysleta a ynstanzias y pedimentos de los vezinos y Padre Procurador Fray Nicolas
Lopez, y la gente y demas socorros que pide para este efecto Testimonio sacado
a la letra de los autos criminales que se fulminaron contra los mansos
apostatas y sus aliados. Contiene quinze foxas escritas y una blanca. Año de
1684 años.
[17] Joe S. Sando was born into the Sun Clan at Jemez Pueblo,
New Mexico. He received his education at Eastern New Mexico University and
Vanderbilt University. His career includes teaching Pueblo history at the
University of New Mexico and ethnohistory at the Institute of American Indian Arts
in Sante Fe. Currently, Sando is Director of Archives, Pueblo Indian Study and
Research Center, at the Pueblo Cultural Center in Albuquerque, New Mexico. He also
serves as consultant to the National Museum of Indian History in Washington,
D.C.
[18] This pueblo was one of
the largest at the time of first contact with the Coronado Expedition in 1541. The
records of the Antonio de Espejo Expedition indicate that the populations might
have been as much as 30,000 in 1583.
[19] He has
published several volumes on the Pueblo including most recently, Pueblo Nations: Eight Centuries of Pueblo Indian History
[Sando 1992], Nee Hemish, A History of the
Jemez Pueblo
[Sando 1982] and The Pueblo Indians
[Sando 1976]. [20] Alfonso Alex Ortiz was born in San Juan Pueblo, New Mexico. He received
his B.A. from the University of New Mexico in 1961 and his M.A. and Ph.D. from the
University of Chicago in 1963 and 1967, respectively. Ortiz's career included
assistant professor at Pitzer College, Claremont, California; professor, Princeton
University, and professor of anthropology at the University of New Mexico. Ortiz's
interests included contemporary Indian affairs, religion and society, and oral
tradition. He also edited Native American writings. He was a Guggenheim Fellow,
1975–76; MacArthur Fellow, 1982–87; and awarded the Indian Achievement Award,
1982. His books include: American Indian Myths and Legends
[Ortiz 1984], New Perspectives on the
Pueblos
[Ortiz 1972], North American Indian
Anthropology: essays on society and culture
[Ortiz 1994], and The Tewa World: Space, Time,
Being and Becoming in a Pueblo Society (Ortiz 1969). [21] An extended version of this table may be found in Appendix 1.
[22] Not included in this work are network-based models in which the
relationship between entities is key. This method of complex system analysis
identifies nodes and linkages between the nodes. The goal is to understand the
interconnectedness of the nodes. Mitchell offers an excellent review of this
process in Chapter 15 of her book. She cites the hyperlinks between web pages, a
neural system, and social relationships as examples [Mitchell 2009, 234]. [23] John Conway’s Game of Life is the most famous of simulations in this
class. See [Cederman 2005]. [24] John von
Neumann and Stanislaw Ulam (1940s) offered a model of life and
self-reproduction.
[25] This modeling simulation was
encoded with the close collaboration of Fernando Sancho Caparrini, University of
Sevilla, Department of Computer Science and Artificial Intelligence. His work on
this modeling project and others is part of a larger research project funded by
the Canadian government to investigate the impact of the Hispanic Baroque as a
cultural system, under the direction of Juan Luis Suárez, Professor of Spanish,
Department of Modern Languages at the University of Western Ontario.
Works Cited
Alessio Robles 1978 Alessio Robles,
Vito. Coahuila y Texas en la época colonial. Mexico
City: Editorial Cultura, 1938; 2nd ed., Mexico City: Editorial Porrúa, 1978.
Archivo General de Indias 1958 Archivo General de Indias, Archivo General de Indias de
Sevilla (Madrid, 1958?)
Arias and Meléndez 2002 Arias, Santa
and Mariselle Meléndez, eds. Mapping Colonial Spanish America: Places and Commonplaces of Identity, Culture,
and Experience. Lewisburg: Bucknell University Press, 2002.
Aschenwald 2002 Aschenwald,
Janette, Stefan Fin, and Gottfried Tappeiner. “Brave New
Modeling: Cellular Automata and Artificial Neural Networks for Mastering
Complexity in Economics.”
Complexity 7:1 (2002): 39–47.
Axelrod and Hammond 2006 Axelrod,
Robert and Ross Hammond. “The Evolution of Ethnocentrism.”
Journal of Conflict Resolutions 50:6 (2006).
Bancroft 1889 Bancroft, H.H.
History of Arizona and New Mexico, 1530–1888. San
Francisco: History Co., 1889.
Bandelier 1910 Bandelier,
Adolphus. “Documentary History of the Rio Grande Pueblos of New
Mexico.” Archeological Institute of America. 13:3 (1910).
Berry 2011 Berry, David M. “The Computational Turn: Thinking about the Digital
Humanities.”
Culture Machine 12 (2011).
Bolton 1916 Bolton, Herbert Eugene.
Spanish Exploration in the Southwest, 1542–1706. New
York: C. Scribner's Sons, 1916.
Boyd and Richerson 2005 Boyd, Robert and
Peter J. Richerson. The Origin and Evolution of
Cultures. New York: Oxford University Press, 2005.
Castaño de Sosa 2004 Castaño de
Sosa, Gaspar. Memoria del descubrimento que Gaspar Castaño de
Sosa, hizo en el Nuevo México, siendo Teniente de Gobernador y Capitán General del
Nuevo Reino de León: (27 de Julio de 1590). 1864. Digital edition.
Alicante: Biblioteca Virtual Miguel de Cervantes, 2004.
Cavalli-Sforza 2000 Cavalli-Sforza,
Luigi. Genes, Peoples, and Languages. Trans. Mark
Seielstad, New York: North Point Press, 2000.
Cavalli-Sforza and Feldman 1981 Cavalli-Sforza, L. L. and M. Feldman. Cultural Transmission and
Evolution. Princeton: Princeton University Press, 1981.
Cederman 2005 Cederman,
Lars-Erik. “Computational Models of Social Forms: advancing
generative Process Theory.”
AJS 110:4 (2005).
Chipman 1992 Chipman, Donald.
Spanish Texas, 1519–1821. Austin: University of Texas
Press, 1992.
Cochinos 2000 Cochinos, Richard.
“Introduction to the Theory of Cellular Automata and
One-Dimensional Traffic Simulation.”
Accessed January 20, 2012.
http://theory.org/complexity/traffic/.
Craddock and Polt 2008 Craddock,
Jerry R. and John H. R. Polt. “The Trial of the Indians of Acoma,
1598–1599.” Berkeley: Cibola Project, Research Center for Romance Studies,
UC Berkeley, 2008.
Dawkins 1978 Dawkins, Richard.
The Selfish Gene. Oxford: Oxford University of Press,
1978 and 2006.
Dawkins 1999 Dawkins, Richard.
The Extended Phenotype. Afterward by Daniel Dennett.
Oxford: Oxford University Press, 1999.
Duaine 1971 Duaine, Carl L. Caverns of Oblivion. Manchaca, Texas: Packrat, 1971.
Flint and Flint 2012 Flint, Richard and
Shirley Flint. Documents of the Coronado Expedition, 1539–1542:
"They Were Not Familiar with His Majesty, nor Did They Wish to Be His
Subjects. Albuequerque: University of New Mexico Press, 2012.
Gabora 1997a Gabora, Liane. “Mind.” Handbook of Archaeological Theories eds. R. A.
Bentley, H. D. G. Maschner, and C. Chippendale, 283–296. Walnut Creek, CA: Altamira
Press, 1997.
Gabora 1997b Gabora, Liane. “The Origin and Evolution of Culture and Creativity.”
Journal of Memetics: Evolutionary
Models of Information Transmission 1:1 (1997).
Hammond and Rey 1953 Hammond,
George P. and Agapito Rey. Don Juan De Oñate, Colonizer of New
Mexico, 1595–1628. 2 vols. Albuquerque: University of New Mexico Press,
1953.
Hammond and Rey 1977 Hammond,
George P. and Agapito Rey, eds. Narratives of the Coronado
Expeditions. Coronado Cuarto Centennial Publications, 1540–1542, v. 2. New
York: AMS Press, 1977.
Holland 1998 Holland, John. Emergence: From Chaos To Order. Cambridge, M.A.: Basic
Books, 1998.
Insula 1999 Insula: Revista de
Ciencias y Humanidades: No. 522, 1999.
Kessell 1989 Kessell, John
Mignolo 1995 Mignolo, Walter.
The Darker Side of the Renaissance: Literacy, Territoriality,
Colonization. Ann Arbor: University of Michigan Press, 1995.
Mignolo 1999 Mignolo, Walter.
Local Histories/Global Designs: Coloniality, Subaltern
Knowledges and Border Thinking. Princeton: Princeton University Press,
1999.
Mitchell 2009 Mitchell, Melanie.
Complexity: A Guided Tour. Oxford, U.K.: Oxford
University Press, 2009.
Morfí 1967 Morfí, Juan Agustín
de.
Diario y derrotero, 1777–1781. Compendio del diario del
viaje a las Provincias Internas [de fray Juan Agustín Morfi]: Chihuahua, 26 Abril
1778, and
History of Texas, 1673–1779. 2
vols. Albuquerque: Quivira Society, 1935; rpt., New York: Arno, 1967.
http://biblioteca.universia.net/autor/Morfi,%20Juan%20Agust%C3%ADn.html
Obregón 1928 Obregón, Baltasar.
Obregón’s History of 16th Century Explorations in Western
America: entitled Chronicle, commentary, or relation of the ancient and modern
discoveries in New Spain and New Mexico, Mexico, 1584. Los Angeles: Wetzel
Publishing Company, 1928.
Ortiz 1984 Ortiz, Alex. American Indian Myths and Legends . New York: Pantheon
Books, 1984.
Ortiz 1994 Ortiz, Alex. North American Indian anthropology: essays on society and
culture. Norman: University of Oklahoma Press, 1994.
Page and Bednar 2007 Page, Scott and
Jenna Bednar. “Can Game(s) Theory Explain Culture? The Emergence
of Cultural Behavior within Multiple Games.”
Rationality and Society 19:1 (2007), 65–97.
Real Academia Española 2001 Real
Academia Española.
Diccionario de la Lengua Española.
22nd edition. 2001.
http://www.rae.es/rae.html
Sando 1976 Sando, Joe. The Pueblo Indians. San Francisco: Indian Historian,
1976.
Sando 1982 Sando, Joe. Nee Hemish: A History of the Jemez Pueblo. Albuquerque:
University of New Mexico Press, 1982.
Sando 1992 Sando, Joe. Pueblo Nations: Eight Centuries of Pueblo Indian History.
Santa Fe: Clear Light Publishers, 1992.
Schroeder and Matson 1965 Schroeder, Albert H. and Dan S. Matson. A Colony on the Move:
Gaspar Castaño De Sosa's Journal, 1590–1591. Santa Fe, New Mexico: School
of American Research, 1965.
Simmons 1993 Simmons, Marc.
Slingerland 2008 Slingerland, Edward. What Science Offers the Humanities:
Integrating Body and Culture. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press,
2008.
Sperber 2007 Sperber, Dan. “Seedless Grapes: Nature and Culture.”
Creations of the Mind: Theories of Artifacts and their
Representations, eds. Stephen Laurence and Eric Margolis. Oxford: Oxford
University Press, 2007.
Sperber and Claidière 2006 Sperber,
Dan and Nicolas Claidière. “Why Modeling Cultural Evolution Is
Still Such a Challenge.”
Biological Theory 1:1 (2006), 20–22.
Thrapp 1991 Thrapp, Dan, ed. Encyclopedia of Frontier Biography. Lincoln: University of
Nebraska Press, 1991.
Toro 1944 Toro, Alfonso. La familia Carvajal: Estudio histórico sobre los judíos y la
Inquisición de la Nueva España en el siglo XVI. 2 vols. Mexico City:
Patria, 1944.
Valcárcel 2010 Valcárcel,
Carmen del Mora. “Las Siete Ciudades de Cíbola: Utopía y Realidad
en la Expedición de Vázquez Coronado.”
El Escritor y el Intelectual Entre Dos Mundos. Madrid:
Iberoamericana-Vervuert Verlag, 2010. 645–658.
Vélez de Escalante and Domínguez 1777 Vélez de Escalante, Silvestre, and Francisco Atanasio Domínguez. Derrotero y diario.
Weddle 1985 Weddle, Robert S. Spanish Sea: The Gulf of Mexico in North American Discovery,
1500–1685. College Station: Texas AandM University Press, 1985.
Wilson and Keil 2001 Wilson, Robert
A. and Frank Keil, eds. MIT Encyclopedia of Cognitive
Sciences. Boston: MIT Press, 2001.
Winship 2009 Winship, George
Parker, Pedro De Castañeda De Najera, Francisco Vásquez de Coronado, Antonio de
Mendoza, and Juan Camilo Jaramillo. The Journey of Coronado,
1540–1542. Santa Barbara: Greenwood Press, 2009.