Abstract
Network representation is a crucial topic in historical social network analysis. The
debate around their value and connotations, led by humanist scholars, is today more
relevant then ever, seeing how common these representations are as support for
historical analysis. Force-directed networks, in particular, are popular as they can
be developed relatively quickly, and reveal patterns and structures in data. The
underlying algorithms, although powerful in revealing hidden patterns, do not retain
meaningful structure and existing hierarchies within historical social networks. In
this article, we question the foreign aspect of this structure that force-directed
layout create in historical datasets. We explore the importance of hierarchy in
social networks, and investigate whether hierarchies - strongly present within our
models of social structure - affect our perception of social network data. Results
from our user evaluation indicate that hierarchical network representations reduce
cognitive load and leads to more frequent and deeper insights into historical social
networks. We also find that users report a preference for the hierarchical graph
representation. We analyse these findings in light of the broader discussion on the
value of force-directed network representations within humanistic research, and
introduce open questions for future work in this line of research.
Introduction
Networks are some of the most commonly found visualizations in digital humanities
research. Used to represent relationships between persons or concepts, this type of
graphs has also been implemented through various layouts to better support specific
tasks and users.
The wide availability of layout algorithms has multiple advantages for researchers
who aim to analyze social networks. Force-directed layouts such as the Gephi Atlas
drastically simplify the visualization – therefore analysis – of large network data.
They reveal patterns and clusters by using complex physical models, all with limited
need for user intervention or mathematical expertise. These models excel in domains
where datasets are large, and where the goal is to identify the structure – or shape
– of the dataset.
However, the widened access to these automated graph drawing algorithms drives us to
interrogate their fitedness for the analysis of humanistic material. On the one hand,
force-directed layouts visually remove existing structures in the data, even when
these structures are meaningful to users and their daily tasks. On the other,
humanistic datasets are often small, but do consist of meaningful inherent clusters,
distances and relationships. Such a mismatch leads to representations that can be at
odds with users' internal perceptions of the data structure. For instance, family
members in a correspondence graph are likely to be spread apart. Similarly,
non-contemporary actors can be placed side to side if they share a common
correspondent. This inconsistency between mental models and visual representation can
become a challenge for users. More critically, it has the potential to impact the
adoption of visualization tools as a whole as research suggests usability and user
trust, rather than scalability, are main obstacles to adoption of digital tools in
the humanities [
Thoden 2017].
In this paper, we address this challenge by introducing
Force-Layered graphs: a novel method for drawing hierarchical graphs
[1] using force-directed
layout. This method uses the existing – and already mature - dynamics of
force-directed algorithms, and injects a meaningful structure into them to organize
them in a way that better supports users’ mental models. We describe the results of a
mixed-methods user study exploring the effect of visual hierarchy for users with
humanities background.
Our initial results reveal that force-layered graph layout outperforms force-directed
layout in terms of number and depth of insights, cognitive load reduction, and user
preference. We discuss these findings along with implications for design and future
directions for this line of research. Specifically, we interrogate the artificial
structure these layouts create, and evaluate whether their disconnect with existing
data hierarchies affects user understanding, preference, and experience.
Structure & Hierarchy in Network Visualization
Discussion about the semantics of visual design elements is often neglected within
technical scholarship on information visualization. In the case of hierarchy, we
believe this discussion to be critical for two reasons. First, because
Human-Computer-Interaction research has shown the importance of the consistency
between users’ mental models of information and its digital representation [
Dørum amd Garland 2011]. Second, because visual hierarchy in network graphs strongly affects their
spatial organization, and spatial position is believed to be one of the most
effective channels in information visualization [
Munzner 2014] [
Bertin 1973]. In
this section, we present a discussion on the semantics of hierarchical trees on one
side, and force-directed networks on the other.
First, we illustrate the difference between the two, through the example of two
visualisations of historical social networks as can be seen in Figure 1. In Figure
1a, a hand-drawn illustration displays the genealogy of the last Byzantine emperor
drawn as a collection of trees. This visualization has a strong spatial organization
based on family lines, roles, and generational hierarchy. In fact, tree structures
can be found in multiple historical representations of networks because they mirror
the conception of lineage as a branching linear connected structure. In Figure 1b, we
see a contemporary representation of a historical network
[3], this time depicting the list of
signatories of a marriage contract in Angoulême, France in the 18th century, as well
as their extended social networks [
Rothschild 2014]. This network, developed using
Gephi shows the global social structure of this historical community, produced using
a force-directed layout, such that highly connected subgroups attract each other,
thereby revealing the cliques and subcommunities within the dataset.
To describe them formally, hierarchical trees (such as Figure 1a) are a sub-category
of networks that are acyclic (i.e. there are no loop encountered during a traversal
of the tree) and directed (i.e. the relationships described by the edges are
one-directional, rather than reciprocal). The hierarchical aspect signifies a certain
categorization of the nodes so that those placed on the same level hold a similar
value or meaning. Hierarchical trees are a spatial organization whose traces date
back to antiquity, and which can be found in a variety of knowledge domains. As
described by Drucker in her book Graphesis [
Drucker 2010], trees are interesting
because their spatial organization carries meaning. In the case of social networks,
this spatial organization becomes an indicator of multiple elements including birth
order, generational breadth and span, patterns of marriage [
Drucker 2010]. Another
interesting aspect to hierarchical trees is their embedded story-telling. The root
node(s), typically placed at the top
[4] of the hierarchy, creates a visual anchor
that drives the reader in, and directs their attention down the edges in a manner
which is almost a narrative approach to visualization.
On the other hand, force-directed networks (Figure 1b) are mathematical products,
where physical simulations ran multiple times on a dataset model attraction and
repulsion between nodes, therefore automatically placing nodes with high connectivity
close to one another, or at the opposite, resulting in disconnected nodes being
placed far from each other, highlighting their disconnect. For Ahnert et al.,
force-directed networks “reject hierarchies”, which become hidden and difficult to
notice, even if they were strongly present within the dataset [
Ahnert et al. 2020]. Modern
readings have argued for the advantages of force-directed networks by justly
questioning the relevance and accuracy of representing the world in rigidly
structured hierarchical roles. In actual historical communities, influence does not
necessarily traverse time and generations in a clear-cut linear way. Similarly, trees
of knowledge fall short when representing complex webs of information, as the
distributed aspect of the web and social networks have clearly illustrated in the
past decades
[5].
In the context of humanistic research, however, the intentional organization of
network data has an undeniable value, when compared to the visual organization that
results of mathematical algorithms. Drucker explains it clearly:
The spatial distribution of network diagrams, topic maps, and other graphical
expressions of processed text or intellectual content is often determined by the
exigencies of screen real estate, rather than by a semantic value inherent in the
visualization. This introduces incidental artifacts of visual information. A point in
a graph may be far from another because of a parameter in the program that governs
display, rather than on account of the weight accorded to the information in the data
set. The argument of the graphic may even be counter to the argument of the
information, creating an interpretative warp or skew, so that what we see and read is
actually a reification of misinformation. [Drucker 2010]
In that way, network visualizations are no different than other visualizations of
data, as they themselves can reproduce existing biases, and cement them as neutral
knowledge. Porras asks us to keep our eyes open to the dangers of network
visualizations as they can “reinscribe historic and contemporary power differentials”
[
Porras 2017].
In visualization literature, a growing voice argues for the consideration of
semantics and connotation in the visualization design, especially within humanistic
scholarship. In previous work [
Lamqaddam et al. 2020], we report how humanists have
consistently commented on the connotative dryness of visualization tools, linking it
to a reduction of rich research material [
Manovich 2012]. Through user research, we
found that humanist scholars felt a
semantic distance between
their experience of their research material and practice and its representation in
visualization tools. Such tools can end up stripping data of essential meaning-making
elements such as temporality, physicality, and terminology. More specifically, we
identified structure in conceptualizations and ontologies as one of the
characteristics of humanistic research that is often missing in visualizations. We
also found proof that meaning, intentionally layered into visualization, had the
potential to reduce the semantic distance experienced by scholars, and alleviate
their discomfort. In this context, the question of representing data’s hierarchical
structures within historical social networks is an interrogation of our Layers of
Meaning (LoM) framework, and an investigation of a larger trend in digital humanities
scholarship during the data era.
Research Questions
To build on the previous discussion, we wanted to investigate the effects that
hierarchical trees and force-directed graphs have on user perception. Because the
literature on mental models in visualization shows link effects on efficiency and
user experience, we developed an extensive user evaluation protocol where we compare
the two representations and investigate the perceptual impacts of each one. In the
next section, we describe the process we followed, and the findings we can extract
from it.
Through this process, we aimed to answer the following research questions:
- RQ1: Do users have an inherent mental model of social network spatial
organizations? How is it visually structured?
- RQ2: How does representing historical social network data using a generational
hierarchy affect cognitive load during social networks analysis tasks?
- RQ3: How does hierarchy within social network data affect insight-building and
recall?
In order to perform a fair comparison, we developed two visualizations showing both
hierarchical and standard force-directed visualizations of historical social
networks. We developed a prototype for hierarchical tree layout visualization using
the Javascript visualization library D3. We chose to develop this algorithm on top of
the existing force-directed layout algorithms for multiple reasons. First, the
existing force-directed layout algorithms are currently mature and deliver
high-quality performance. Perhaps as a consequence, these layouts are supported by
major graph drawing libraries and toolkits, which opens future possibilities for
wider implementation of hierarchical graph layouts. Force-directed layouts also
include parameters to reduce node collision, occlusion, and bring in useful results,
such as bringing together highly connected clusters, which, while we do not want it
to break inherent hierarchy in the data, still proves to be useful in reducing
edge-crossing.
We designed and implemented an algorithm that traverses a force-directed graph and
spatially organizes it based on a user-defined parameter. The full algorithm
description and code can be found on our
GitHub page.
Figure 2 shows the same dataset represented as
both a force-directed graph (left), and a hierarchical network (right). We call the
graphs produced by our hierarchical layout
force-layered graphs.
This porte-manteau hints at the underlying force-directed layout, and includes
the layered aspect of hierarchical graphs (alternatively referred to as layered
graphs, or Sugiyama-graphs).
Study
In this section, we describe the evaluation protocol used to answer the RQs described
in section 3. In HCI, user evaluations are a standard way to get feedback and to get
better insight into users’ needs. It is also a common way to evaluate usability and
identify obstacles to adoption of digital tools. Since usability and adoption are a
recurrent issue in DH research, there is real value to be gained from applying such
methods to pinpoint exact usability issues in collaboration with the intended
audience. In this section, we describe each step of the methodology together with the
results found. For each subsection, the full table of results can be found in the
annex. In this section, we sometimes use the notations F-D and
F-L as shorthand for Force-Directed
or Force-Layered graphs respectively.
Participants. We recruited 15 participants (12 women, 3 men) with background in Art
History and Digital Humanities, most of them graduate students in the university’s
Digital Humanities MSc program. Participants had on average 3.6 years of experience
in their domain, and 1.2 years of experience using digital tools to support their
research or study practice. Among participants, most digital tool usage within field
of study consisted of digital archives (11 out of 15), spreadsheets (10 out of 15),
and graph visualization tools (9 out of 15).
Data. We used data extracted from the Cornelia
[6]
database, which uses archival documents to gather historical data on 17th century
artistic communities
[7]. We selected a subset containing 38 persons related through
ancestry, marriage and godparenthood links. This dataset specifically highlights
(extra)familial hierarchy structure.
Graphs. Figure 3 shows the two network visualization used in this study. We used the
same dataset for both graphs not to introduce bias. However, to minimize learning
effects, we manually changed the names so that the similarity of the two networks
would not be obvious.
Mental model elicitation
Understanding users' existing mental models (i.e. the representation they
internally make of specific information structures or processes) is key if we aim
to support similar structures in building visualizations. Previous work in the HCI
field has demonstrated the value of asking participants to draw their mental
models as a way to externalize them [
Zhang 2008] [
Kodama et al. 2017].
Before showing participants the graphs, we first started by asking each one to
freely draw a social structure of their choosing, in order to elicit their mental
mapping of social structures. They were invited to sketch out any fictional or
existing social structure network (from stories, movies or books, or their own
personal life
[8]). They were
encouraged to consider lineage relationships, but also non-blood ties such as
extended families, and close friends. We then extracted the recurring themes
present in the produced sketches. A few of these sketches can be seen in Figure
4.
We found that in all
[9] cases,
participants sketched graphs had a hierarchical structure. These representations
were mainly tree structures, with many of them structured as layered graphs. We
also found that most participants used the vertical axis as a single indicator of
generational division. However, in some cases, this pattern was broken as
different relationships (edges towards friends for instance) were represented as
orthogonal to the family links (Figure 4, P15). Indeed, almost a third (35\%) of
participants chose to represent multiple relationship types within the graphs.
Familiar structure, or expression of an internal mental model? While we can use
the results from this elicitation task to induce the existence of a ‘natural’
hierarchical mental model of such data for the users, the exposure to family-tree
type representations is an equally likely explanation for the phenomenon. Family
trees are an old form of representations that are commonly seen by the public
[Mitchell, 2014]. They are also commonly used in (art) historical teaching, which
makes our participants even more likely to have built familiarity towards them. In
reality, our participants have likely interacted with similar representations in
the past, and may have defaulted to recreating a similar structure. We consider
that these results do inform us of the internal model within the participants,
itself caused by either inherent hierarchization of this data, or unvoluntary
training caused by previous exposure.
Think aloud study
We asked participants to explore the dataset in each condition while vocalizing
their thought process. Similar to the mental model elicitation approach, this
method allows to make the thought process of participants explicit. We then
captured and transcribed participants think-aloud process and analyzed it using a
thematic analysis to uncover patterns of insight
[10] and
identify differences between the force-directed and the force-layered
conditions.
We found that the force-layered visualization triggered more interpretative
[11] and reflective insights. We also find more occurrences of
intrigue or curiosity about the portrayed social structures (
“
It's too bad you can't see more about the person, you can't tell for example
what their profession is,” “I don't know the person, but I imagine if I were
researching these people, it can be really interesting to see”). Some
participants also found contradictions in the underlying data, and offered
hypotheses to explain those, or simply pointed them out (“[This person]
has an offspring but it's somehow not an
event for [their wife]
— it's not an event for her,
although it is with her"”). Finally, we found a clear divide in the number
of negative emotions such as overwhelmingness or defeat in the condition of
force-directed network usage (
“There are a lot of persons, and
here a lot of events. I’m trying to see who’s linked to who but it’s really
difficult here.”, “
I don’t like this one. Yeah it’s really confusing and I
don’t find … ugh [trails off]”).
Exploration Tasks
To complement the open nature of the think-aloud exploration, we conducted a more
structured exploration task where we asked participants to answer specific
questions about the social dynamics portrayed in the visualization. We designed
these questions based on the existing literature on graph exploration tasks
[12] and categorized them into easy questions (e.g. “
find the most connected actor”, “
Find all
people who knew both X and Y”) and difficult questions (“
How does the graph evolve between birth of person X and their death”,
“Describe the network at a specific year”).
We assessed the answers to the exploration tasks based on complete and partial
correctness. We found that for easy questions, a similar number of participants
(F-D: n=11; F-L: n=10) gave correct answers with both conditions. However, in the
force-directed condition, half of these answers were incomplete. To the difficult
questions, more participants gave correct answers using the force-layered
condition (n=6). Notably, however, none of the participants were able to answer
the same questions using the force-directed condition completely and correctly.
The number of participants who gave incomplete answers was the same for both
conditions.
Perception Task
In order to measure how the perception of the graphs compared with their actual
characteristics, we collected participants recollection of some quantitative graph
characteristics such as network size, and number of generations. We also asked
about the perceived complexity of the social structure. Both networks contained
the same number of nodes thirty-eight nodes. We found that participants perceived
the force-layered graph to contain fewer nodes (median=30)
than was the case. They were more slightly accurate when using the force-directed
graph (median=35).
Finally, a majority of participants reported feeling that the social structure
displayed in the force-layered condition was of a simpler nature one than the
force-directed. This corroborates the difference in perceived graph size, as
participants perceived networks in the force-layered condition to have fewer
nodes.
Recall task
To assess recall, we aimed to see how well participants remembered the graph in
its entirety. We then asked them to draw the graph to their best recollection
after having diverted their attention for a brief moment
[13]. We then analyzed the resulting sketch
based on categories including the restitution of meaningful sub-communities,
node-link structure, and visual detail.
We found differences in specific metrics between the recall sketches of the two
versions, specifically on the identification of bridge nodes, as well as the
retention of the multiple types of relationships. In the force-directed recall
sketches, participants have retained the “dog-bone” structure of the graph
consisting of two large clusters representing the two main families and tied
together through a bridge node. Most participants also retained - at least
partially - the lower antennas formed by the two loosely connected pairs nodes. In
the force-layered condition, the drawings have retained the left-to-right
organization of the nodes, as well as the two distinct subgraphs representing the
two families. The bridge node bringing these two groups together was not
systematically included. Finally, participants retained the existence of multiple
types of relationships better in the force-layered condition, as they recalled
more of the godparent type links. Example drawings from participants for each
condition can be seen in Figure 5.
Cognitive load
While evaluating performance can provide a good understanding of efficiency
achieved using a system, the mental effort required is not answered by these
approaches. We therefore measure participants' cognitive load
[14] when using the two conditions.
As a measure for cognitive load, we used a raw NASA-TLX questionnaire [Hart, 1998]
The NASA-TLX questionnaire is a widely used metric for measuring cognitive load in
the HCI field. It consists of a self-reported scale capturing workload across six
dimensions: Mental, Physical, and Temporal Demands, Frustration, Effort, and
Performance [Hart, 1998]. Participants are asked to score each of these variables
on a 0 — 100 scale where a lower value represents a lower demand
[15].
Comparing the raw NASA-TLX scores, we find that the force-layered visualization
(average score of 26.3) was found to be less demanding than the force-directed
graph (average score of 35.78), the metrics with most variance between conditions
being mental demand, effort, and frustration (see Figure 6).
User preference
Finally, we performed a preference-based ranking. Participants were asked to
subjectively rank the force-directed and the force-layered graphs on a Likert
scale on aspects such as perceived suitability to task and data, or visual appeal.
Figure 7 shows the average results of the preference rankings for each category.
Globally, participants ranked the force-layered condition higher than the
force-directed condition in suitability to both data and tasks. They indicated
that the force-layered condition needed less effort to understand than the
standard one, and that they had higher confidence in their answers. Finally, the
force-layered graphs also scored higher in visual appeal, and overall preference.
Results & Discussion
Based on a task of mental model elicitation, we found that participants
overwhelmingly relied on generational hierarchical structure to draw familial social
networks. This can be either because of inherent structure perceived in the data, or
association of this type of dataset with traditional ancestry tree representations.
When exploring two different versions of a social network where one is layered
hierarchically and the other one isn’t, we found that participants expressed more –
and deeper – insights in the case of the force-layered visualization. They also
engaged more critically with the dataset, for instance by pointing out
inconsistencies. The force-directed version, however, drew more comments and
descriptions about the visualization features, suggesting that participants engaged
more with the visualization in the force-directed condition,
and with the data in the force-layered condition. We also
found that the force-layered representation elicited more positive emotions such as
curiosity and expressions of enjoyment or interest, that were not as frequent using
the force-directed version. Moreover, our results reveal that users perceived the
social structure shown in force-layered graphs to be smaller, and less complex than
similar structure visualized through a force-directed layout. However, they were more
accurate in size estimation using the force-directed layout.
Concerning recall, we found that participants retained more of the significant
clusters with the force-layered condition, although they retained more of the bridge
nodes after using the force-directed version. In both cases, the bridge nodes were
placed in the center of the visualization, so saliency does not explain the
difference in these results. We suggest that can be caused by the topology of each
graph. In the force-layered version, the ties among each family create two horizontal
substructures that become the major ground of the visual, while the dog-bone
structure in the force-directed layout represent the major part of the ground figure,
and the bridge node is an essential part to its structure. Finally, using a raw
NASA-TLX questionnaire and a preference ranking, we found that participants found the
force-layered visualization less demanding cognitively. They also judged it to be
more suited to the tasks and data, and more visually appealing.
As we analyze these findings in conversation with the literature on visualization in
humanistic research, we identify emerging discussion points:
- Engagement with data & engagement with visualization. In the think-aloud
study results, we describe how hierarchical trees seemingly elicit an engagement
with the data, while force-directed layouts trigger
comments about the features and characteristics of the tool
itself. This subtle distinction has relevant implications, as it suggests that the
spatial organization of the network affects the perception of the data
representation. Visualized data can therefore be integrated in users’ mental
models in a way that allows them to analyze and question it; or it can be obscured
by the layer of foreign conceptualization that does not allow it to blend with
their existing knowledge. As it is, it is critical to encourage representations
that allow a connection with the research material itself, and where the
visualization features themselves do not become the center of attention for users.
We also see this finding as a validating step of the guidelines developed in the
Layers of Meaning framework [Lamqaddam et al. 2020]. In this case, by layering meaning
through the structure of network graphs, we find that there is an effect to the
level of engagement users have with research data. What seems to be a slight
visual difference appears to affect the very focus of users’ attention, which in
turn reveals the critical aspect of connotative elements and intentional design
choices in visualization.
- Force-directed network structure as artifact. In the analysis of the recall of
social structure information, we note that more participants recalled the
existence of a bridge node between the two clusters with the force-directed
network. We suggest that this is due to the singular topology of the generated
network. In truth, what is a particular visual characteristic of a network
topology (a bridge node between two tight clusters) is an abstraction of a
significant piece of information in the dataset (a common person bringing together
two large families). In a way, the fact that the force-directed layout created
this artificial shape helped highlight a very important information in the data,
which has been better remembered by participants. From this perspective, we can
thus defend that force-directed layouts also create meaning
through the specific topologies they generate. These shapes are often singular, or
unique, because they are not a product of intentional design. Rather, then come as
the result of computations unique to the dataset, the canvas space and the set
parameters. We therefore defend the concept of a force-directed network
organization as an artifact in itself. One that holds the potential to carry
meaning; to be recognized, analyzed, and categorized.
- Defending the importance of challenging mental models. We argue throughout this
article that one of the strengths of hierarchical tree structures is that they
reproduce existing mental models, therefore making it easier to read and grasp
information they represent as it fits within scholars’ existing knowledge.
However, existing models of knowledge are not necessarily correct, accurate, or
good to perpetuate. For instance, conventional family trees carry a variety of
assumptions about social structures that make them both limited and limiting when
representing real-life data. Worse, these representations, when uncritically
absorbed, function as prescriptive principles of how communities should exist in
society. There is therefore an undeniable larger implication in the discussion
about how social network data should be represented. The improved understanding,
preference and lower effort that hierarchical trees bring needs to be balanced –
or perhaps complemented - by the novel perspectives that force-directed layouts
create.
Implications & Future work
More research is needed to assess the effect of other types of structure in network
visualizations. Once these effects are clearly understood, it would be interesting to
evaluate how novel graph layout such as our FL algorithm can be introduced to
existing visualization packages. It would also be interesting to make them
customizable to specific needs outside of family and generational structures. Indeed,
an important implication of this work is that any singular off-the-shelf technique
cannot be sufficient for a solid support of DAH research. Rather than promote the use
of a specific visualization technique over another, we advocate for researchers
access to a variety of representation techniques that can accompany different tasks,
data types, and expertise levels.
Finally, the generalizability of our discussion to other humanistic fields is an
additional avenue for future research. Since this study was mainly focused on an art
historical research, our initial results cannot be readily extrapolated to different
domains. However, similar challenges of historical social networks exist for humanist
researchers across disciplines. Future research is needed to evaluate how hierarchy
in historical social network visualizations affects different research fields
depending on their unique needs and constraints.
Conclusion
In this article, we explored the perceptual effect of hierarchy in visualizations of
historical social networks. We have found, based on a user-centered approach, that
humanist participants had a hierarchical mental model of social networks, indicating
that there could be advantages to representing social networks in a similarly
hierarchical structure. Our study confirmed this hypothesis, as we found that users
reported lower cognitive load, more frequent and deeper insights, as well as a
significant preference for the hierarchical representations. Despite these results,
we nuance these findings by calling attention to the fact that force-directed graphs
can carry meaning in their generated topology, allow users to make conclusions on
tightness of networks, clusters, and question the accepted dogmas in terms of
community structure. However, because social networks are so critical to certain
humanistic research domains, the perceptual benefits to hierarchically structured
layouts should not be overlooked. As graph-drawing software is often the most
accessible way for researchers to gather quick visual overview, we suggest that these
layouts should be more commonly supported as alternatives to allow scholars to have
multiple perspectives on their data.
By these findings, we hope to contribute to the necessary critical discussion
nuancing the role of network visualization in humanistic research. In the future, we
would like to broaden the scope of this study by investigating the effects of
hierarchy for other types of historical (social) networks
[16]. We believe such an
interdisciplinary line of research has the potential to trigger novel questions – for
instance about the impact of structure in network visualization of historical data,
and the perceptual effects of commonly used design patterns. Only by questioning what
we assume to be effective can we (re)design visualization tools that blend in with
scholars’ experience of their material and practice, and ultimately strengthen the
value of visualization as a research tool for humanists.
Acknowledgements
This research is part of Project Cornelia, a project funded by the University of
Leuven (KU Leuven) and the Flemish Fund for Scientific Research-Belgium
(FWO-Vlaanderen). We thank our colleagues from AugmentHCI (Department of Computer
Science, KU Leuven) and Project Cornelia (Faculty of Arts, KU Leuven) who gave
feedback and insight into this study. We are also deeply grateful to the entire
NA+DAH team for supporting and encouraging this line of research, and particularly to
Dr. John Ladd (Denison University) for reviewing and strengthening this manuscript.
Notes
[1]
also known as hierarchical or Sugiyama-style graphs.
[2] This layout is a specific type of force-directed layout
that confines nodes to a circular space. The Fruchterman-Reingold layout on
Gephi is an example of such algorithms.
[3] We defaulted to a
contemporary representation for the non-hierarchical layout as these can only be
found in recent representations. See [Freeman2004] for a history of network
representation of social structures. [4] Or at the right, in a right-to-left
horizontal spatial organization
[5] Pushed to its extreme, this urge to categorize the world can be
linked to historical movements which have led to similar attempts at categorizing
peoples and communities, thereby creating and intensifying the construction of
race for example, solidified through its representation as formal scientific
truth. This discussion, while outside the scope of this article, cannot be ignored
when discussing the role of hierarchy and knowledge in human history.
[6] Projectcornelia.be
[7] Specifically Flemish painters and tapestry
producers.
[8] We asked participants to anonymize any personal information in
the case they wanted to draw their personal social structure, in order to
maintain privacy for them and their social circles.
[9] We excluded a single sketch from our analysis (P11) as
it represented an evolutionary tree rather than a social network.
[10] In order to categorize the
insight level of participants, we used the framework defined by Claes et al.
[Claes and Moere 2015] distinguishing three levels of insight depth: factual insight,
referring to a description of visible data, interpretive insight, where data is
synthesized along with additional objective or experienced information, then
reflective, which also includes subjective or emotional response. [11]
In our study, factual insight describes statements such as “he dies in 1668”
or “this person has children.” Interpretative insight includes “it's just
close family I see,” “This person has children, but not with this one I
think.” Finally, in reflective insight, we included comments about the data
quality (“I got the feeling this [dataset] is more documented than the other
one”) and emotional engagement with the data (“oh they had *many* children
*laughs*”).
[12]
We used the taxonomy proposed by Lee et al. [Lee et al. 2006] and partly aimed
towards evaluators who wish to compare graph visualizations. From a domain
point of view, we looked at Lamqaddam et al. task categorization [Lamqaddam 2018], which is more specific to the domain of family tree visualization in the
humanities. [13] We used this time
to ask participants to fill out the NASA-TLX questionnaire, described in the
next paragraph. This step had the advantage to distract participants from the
screen and the graph itself, while not being too distracting that it led them
to unrelated trains of thought.
[14] Cognitive load
is a multidimensional construct representing mental effort – or load imposed on
the cognitive system by performing a certain task. Evaluation of this measure
has been found to be useful to complement performance-based evaluations of
effectiveness in visualization.
[15] In the full
version of a NASA-TLX, participants are also asked to weigh these variables
depending on their perceived relevance to the task. The final score of a full
NASA-TLX is a weighed average of the user score based on the overall weight
distribution. However, many researchers report using a raw version of the test, where the scores are simply averaged with no
consideration of weights. Since little difference in sensitivity has been found
between raw and weighed TLX, we decided for a raw-TLX to keep evaluation time
short.
[16] Within the Cornelia
database, hierarchical relations such as those between masters and apprentices in
a workshop are good candidates for such an exploration.
Works Cited
Ahnert et al. 2020 Ahnert, R., Ahnert, S.E., Coleman, C.N. and Weingart, S.B., 2020. The Network Turn: Changing Perspectives in the Humanities. Cambridge University Press.
Bertin 1973 Bertin, J. (1973). “Sémiologie graphique: les diagrammes, les réseaux, les cartes.” De Gruyter Mouton.
Bishop 2018 Bishop, C. (2018). “Against digital art history.” International
Journal for Digital Art History, (3).
Claes and Moere 2015 Claes, S., & Moere, A. V. (2015). “The role of tangible interaction in
exploring information on public visualization displays.” In Proceedings of the 4th International Symposium on Pervasive Displays (pp.
201-207).
Drucker 2010 Drucker, J. (2010). “Graphesis: Visual knowledge production and representation.” Poetess Archive Journal, 2(1), 1-50.
Dørum amd Garland 2011 Dørum, K., & Garland, K. (2011). “Efficient electronic navigation: A metaphorical
question?.” Interacting with Computers, 23(2), 129-136.
Freeman2004 Freeman, L. (2004). The development of social network analysis. A
Study in the Sociology of Science, 1(687),
159-167.
Hart and Staveland 1988 Hart, S. G., & Staveland, L. E. (1988). “Development of NASA-TLX (Task Load
Index): Results of empirical and theoretical research.” In Advances
in psychology (Vol. 52, pp. 139-183). North-Holland.
Kienle 2017 Kienle, M. (2017). “Between nodes and edges: Possibilities and limits of network
analysis in art history.” Artl@ s Bulletin, 6(3), 1.
Kodama et al. 2017 Kodama, C., Jean, B. S., Subramaniam, M., & Taylor, N. G. (2017). “There’s a creepy
guy on the other end at Google!: engaging middle school students in a drawing
activity to elicit their mental models of Google.” Information
Retrieval Journal, 20(5), 403-432.
Kurgan 2013 Kurgan, L. (2013). “Close up at a distance: Mapping, technology, and politics.” MIT Press.
Lamqaddam 2018 Lamqaddam, H., Aerts, J., Brosens, K., & Verbert, K. (2018). “Nahr: A
visual representation of social networks as support for art history research.” In Proceedings of the 10th Nordic Conference on Human-Computer
Interaction (pp. 634-641).
Lamqaddam et al. 2020 Lamqaddam, H., Moere, A. V., Abeele, V. V., Brosens, K., & Verbert, K. (2020).
“Introducing Layers of Meaning (LoM): A Framework to Reduce Semantic Distance of
Visualization In Humanistic Research.” IEEE Transactions on
Visualization and Computer Graphics, 27(2), 1084-1094.
Lee et al. 2006 Lee, B., Plaisant, C., Parr, C. S., Fekete, J. D., & Henry, N. (2006). “Task
taxonomy for graph visualization.” In Proceedings of the 2006 AVI
workshop on BEyond time and errors: novel evaluation methods for information
visualization (pp. 1-5).
Manovich 2012 Manovich, L. (2012). “Museum without walls, art history without names: visualization
methods for Humanities and Media Studies.” Software Studies
Initiative.
Munzner 2014 Munzner, T. (2014). “Visualization analysis and design.” CRC
press.
Porras 2017 Porras, S. (2017). “Keeping our eyes open: Visualizing networks and art history.” Artl@ s Bulletin, 6(3), 3.
Thoden 2017 Thoden, K., Stiller, J., Bulatovic, N., Meiners, H.L. and Boukhelifa, N., 2017.
“User-centered design practices in digital humanities–experiences from dariah and
cendari.” ABI Technik, 37(1),
pp.2-11.
Zhang 2008 Zhang, Y. (2008). “The influence of mental models on undergraduate students’ searching
behavior on the Web.” Information Processing & Management,
44(3), 1330-1345.