Abstract
The Digital Humanities and Literary Cognition (DHLC) lab at Michigan State
University was founded in 2012 with the mission of bringing together scholars
from disciplines including literature, neuroscience, education, computer
science, and digital humanities to investigate the cognitive processes involved
in reading texts of all kinds. The real story is much more complicated: emerging
through a combination of institutional risk-taking and serendipitous accident,
the lab was — and remains — an example of how conventional definitions of a
“laboratory,” and particularly the rather fraught label
of “humanities lab,” often come short of capturing the true
cross-disciplinary nature of this type of work. More than any academic or
methodological template, the DHLC lab emerged through the curiosity and
enthusiasm of the students who inhabit it. For that reason, this case study — a
brief history of the DHLC’s development, cultural practices, and ongoing work —
has been written collaboratively with multiple voices, including the lab
director (Natalie Phillips) and the student researchers. Together, we describe
the challenges and opportunities created by embarking on a radically
interdisciplinary research effort. We report the elements that enabled the DHLC
lab to find success through a willingness to abandon expectations and adapt to
rapidly evolving research practices, while maintaining our core focus on the
humanities. We hope this case study proves useful for those who, like us, seek
to explore the vast potential of interdisciplinary lab space for DH practice,
the pedagogical benefits of collaborative research, and how to prioritize
humanities in a STEM-focused world.
Prologue
The Digital Humanities and Literary Cognition (DHLC) lab at
Michigan State University (MSU) emerged not through top-down strategic
planning, but through a combination of university proactivity and
serendipitous accident, with an emphasis on the latter.[1] It emerged organically from
interactions between me and excited students, newly interested MSU
colleagues, collaborators from Duke and Stanford... and at times, people who
just happened to be walking by and came in. This odd beginning is precisely
what makes our lab such an interesting case study, similar to the
development of other humanities labs [Breithaupt 2017]. One
common way to approach building such a lab — a trend you might say — is by
creating a digital scholarship or digital humanities center, a space devoted
to supporting as many different fields, departments, and faculty as
possible: building websites, “provid[ing] a support
mechanism for the growing areas of e-research and digital
scholarship,” and facilitating the use of big, expensive pieces
of technology (or even better, the analysis of “big
data”) [Lippincott 2014]. We have a version of this
at MSU, the Digital
Scholarship Lab, and it’s amazing. That’s not quite what we do. I
can run an MRI and conduct advanced brain imaging, but I can’t build a
website to save my life, so I’m glad that others aren’t relying on me or the
lab to do this type of DH. Instead, the DHLC began with a specific research
question about close reading and literary neuroscience. Now, we design and
run a variety of new experiments, opening up the space for ever-growing
grassroots engagement with others across campus and with our
community.
In 2012, the lab wasn’t what most would call a
“lab.” One day, I was working with three students —
one from English, one from education, one from neuroscience — at a table
we’d dragged from a supply closet. Clustered over our laptops, we were so
focused we scarcely looked up as a team of technicians walked in and began
installing computers around us. For a DH lab, the moment when the technology
appears is normally a glorious occasion; looking back, I can see why the
arrival of these computers wasn’t treated with the usual tech-centric
revelry. The actual lab was already there — in full swing, fueled by the
energy being brought by everyone around the table. The lab was the students,
the people. And with them, we got momentum, a combination of student energy
and brilliance that would propel us for the next seven years.
It’s the energy of the people in the lab that sustains me as I
navigate the balance of my own work and these new fields we’re engaging. I’m
an eighteenth centuryist who gets excited about Margaret Cavendish’s
seventeenth-century jokes about the brain, and I also put (willing!)
students in brain scanners to see if we can learn more about the cognitive
intricacy and uniqueness of reading literature. Fortunately, I work
alongside equally interdisciplinary folks. The computer scientist who helped
us code our sonnet study at MSU loves poetry so much he actually memorizes
it, down to the punctuation (you should hear him recite Whitman or Keats).
My co-lead on our music study was a major in violin before he switched to
computer science; only later did he jump to cognitive science, focusing on
rhythm and music. This sort of interdisciplinary work is not without its
limits. As Urszula Pawlicka points out with Bruno Latour, just the name
“humanities lab” risks assuming an ideologically
dangerous “scientification of the humanities.” If
the laboratory is “a place that has been socially
constructed so as to project the mythos of science onto the world
outside its walls,” she writes, “then
humanities laboratories may begin by borrowing the foreign glamour of
science and end by reproducing its ideological parochialisms”
[Pawlicka 2017, 527]. Rather than remaining naïve to such
challenges, our lab — with over thirty students from across disciplines —
tackles them head-on in an ongoing, energetic attempt to demonstrate that
one can do work that cuts across both humanities and sciences without caving
to reductionism and instead centering our humanist commitments. We have not
given in to an “interdisciplinary delusion” but seek to
model the pluralism of true cross-disciplinary work, with all the
multiplicity it entails [Kramnick 2018]. This is why we are
writing this case study — and why we have chosen to tell it collectively.
The only way to tell this story is to have students write as well, as equal
collaborators and researchers who have made the lab what it is
today.
Introduction
The sounds of the lab on any given day include the clack of typing, the light
squeak of pen against dry-erase board, and music — live streaming everything
from classical to indie-rock (one undergrad lab lead’s favorite band is Rainbow
Kitten Surprise). Academic conversations about our latest lab project on music
and narrative intermix with light cursing over how to change font size in
E-Prime (the requisite software for taking our poetry study to fMRI). It’s a
space for kvetching about grad school applications. Most importantly, it’s a
space for laughter, be it a fierce debate on whether the lab’s next event will
be an escape room or laser tag, or an equally fierce argument over whose
favorite candy is actually disgusting. Entering the lab means joining a warm,
rich, chaotic space full of work, odd debates, and enjoyment. The collegial and
even goofy atmosphere of the lab is not a weakness — if anything, it energizes
our work and makes it more generative. This is our kind of
interdisciplinarity.
One of the real strengths of an interdisciplinary and comparatively young field
like digital humanities is the sheer amount of experimentation and invention
that digital humanists engage in on a daily basis. We have to, because the work
we do is often the first of its kind. We can’t help but be cutting-edge, unique,
and integrative, because we have limited models to draw from in terms of
literary neuroscience. Even as digital humanities labs at prestigious
universities such as Stanford, MIT, and CUNY have become well established, there
remain many interdisciplinary areas (i.e. literary cognition) that are only just
starting to be explored. As the field continues to grow and mature, however, we
have the opportunity to build on one another’s wisdom, to create new models and
best practices for present and future digital humanities scholars to follow.
That is part of what this article seeks to do, inviting readers to learn from
the story of our challenges as well as our successes.
We cannot, however, give you a template. We do not have a model you can simply
copy. There is no formula for how to create your own cutting-edge humanities lab
— such a thing does not exist, because the most effective labs and programs
build on the specific strengths of a given community. Instead, the following
case study it is an exploration of one lab, our Digital Humanities and Literary
Cognition (DHLC) lab, with a discussion of some of the approaches and strategies
we used to create it. In our current moment when the question is shifting from
whether or not to have humanities labs to how those labs should operate in more
inclusive, equitable, and sustainable ways, our experiences in the DHLC offer
some practices for those working in labs old, new, and yet to be. As you read
the specifics of how the DHLC works and why we set it up that way, we invite you
to imagine yourself and your own situation — to dream of what your own lab could
be.
The Digital Humanities and Literary Cognition (DHLC) lab was founded in 2012. Its
first seven years provide a powerful and unique case study that reveals what
interdisciplinary work in DH can be, including points of promise and innovation,
points of disciplinary tension and translation, and most importantly, lessons
that point to the power of actively involving students in a humanities lab. At
its core, the DHLC’s mission is to understand how humans think in and through
genres of literary art. In other words, we want to understand the intertwined
cognitive, neurological, emotional, and cultural processes by which we engage
with art, relate to it, and use it to make sense of ourselves. We call our
research in cognitive and digital humanities “literary
neuroscience,” a phrase that captures how such work could never be
just one or the other, just humanities or just science.
In 1959, C.P. Snow famously described the humanities and sciences as “two cultures” separated by a “gap” of “mutual incomprehension.” It
seemed the two were unable to communicate, and the increasingly siloed nature of
disciplinary knowledge throughout the twentieth century did little to help. The
present situation is a paradox. As many institutions call for interdisciplinary
scholarship and teaching, arts and humanities programs continue to be defunded
[
Wythoff 2018]. In this cultural moment, interdisciplinary
labs like the DHLC have a vital role to play by communicating across the
humanities and sciences, translating the value of the humanities, and in
particular, championing the critical perspective that the humanities
brings to interdisciplinary research.
The DHLC works hard to be a truly interdisciplinary space — we actively integrate
methodologies, approaches, and scholars from different disciplines on equal
footing. As one example, we always insist on capturing both qualitative and
quantitative data whenever we design experiments, and we further ensure that our
qualitative data includes space for narrative. The stories and reflections of
our study participants are as important as the numbers on an aesthetic pleasure
scale, and our student researchers specializing in both humanistic and
scientific methods work directly with each other in small groups to analyze the
data in tandem. In this way we bring together the strengths of both sciences and
humanities, which gives us a richer set of data that enables us to talk to
people in different fields in their own languages and concepts.
In pursuing such a significant cultural and academic goal, it can be easy to get
lost in the sauce, so we try to keep our day-to-day research practices in
perspective. Walking into the DHLC means walking into a space enclosed by the
usual four walls, one of which is a window that stretches from floor to ceiling,
offering a gorgeous view of trees and the occasional sunset. Double layered,
bright green blinds — technologically advanced to move with the changing
daylight, yet broken beyond comprehension — seem to have a mind of their own and
close and open over the window randomly, making temperature regulation uniquely
difficult. To counteract this, we have an ancient box fan, named “Fanny” after the protagonist of Mansfield Park. Eight state-of-the-art computers for processing MRI
data, DH visualizations, and project results line two walls, and tables are
littered with brightly colored sticky notes that range from complex mathematical
equations to single words like “gender?!?” There are
four different styles of chairs, ranging from the foldable to the “spinny” chairs that allow for a dramatic turn to listen
to someone talking. A portrait of Jane Austen looks over the lab, observing the
years of inside jokes written around the room. Most importantly, this physical
room we call our lab — the DHLC — not only brings together students from
English, Psychology, Neuroscience, Computer Science, Rhetoric, Business, and
Art; it creates unique points of contact between them. Neuroscience students
read poetry, debate Austen’s irony, and listen to excerpts of orchestral music.
Scholars of literature and psychology work together on interdisciplinary fMRI
experiment design while artists join computer scientists to develop new digital
tools for making art accessible across museum spaces, digital platforms, and
coffee shops. There are not many places where a freshman undergrad in political
science can talk literary cognition theory with a 5th year English PhD, or where
you find professors taking careful notes about sophomores and seniors’ latest
project insights. But this is what it means to be at the DHLC.
Our lab has never adhered to traditional hierarchies, and we actively cultivate a
lab culture that resists the neoliberal, top-down power structures of the
contemporary university [
Lothian 2013]
[
Mann 2019]. We have a PI and lab leads, but these roles are more
administrative than anything else. Questions, ideas, answers, and new directions
can, should, and do come from everyone (including new researchers bringing fresh
perspective to our projects), such that it can be easy to forget who is an
undergraduate student, who is a graduate student, and who is a faculty member.
Our PI specifically goes by “Natalie” in the lab as one way
to emphasize our non-hierarchical structure. No one asks your rank when you walk
into a lab meeting, nor offers their own; instead, anyone might suggest where
you can jump in on the latest spreadsheet or data sorting. Our
interdisciplinarity supports this setup, because we recognize that each of our
members brings a different set of skills and experiences to the lab. Strict
hierarchies simply do not make sense when a third-year student might be the
expert on a piece of software, a first-year student might be trained in fMRI, a
graduate student might have a music theory background, a faculty member might
have several successful grants under her belt, and a fourth-year student might
have actually run participants in a particular study. We help each other
communicate across these many fields and experiences by holding workshops during
our regular meetings where researchers will introduce each other to particular
concepts, theories, methods, or software and discuss how to best incorporate
that knowledge in our studies. In essence, we all do the work, we all have
different roles to play, and we could not possibly do all that the lab does if
we assumed that any of those roles is more prestigious or more important.
Bringing together science and humanities in the study of cognition and literary
art is certainly a tall order. We “manage” it in the lab —
day by day and project by project — by relying on an open system and a
question-led approach, rather than an institutionally-designed order. This model
brings with it particular affordances and limitations. It requires maintaining
an open lab space in collaboration with student researchers and project leads
while also ensuring secure storage for study participants’ data. It relies
heavily on the energy and guidance of both the PI and the lab leads. It invites
students from all kinds of disciplines who never dreamed of being part of a lab
to join in the work of creating knowledge. It means that projects are abandoned
partway through, or unnecessarily redone, or go in three directions at once; it
also leads to astonishing breakthroughs. It loses some portion of institutional
knowledge with every student researcher’s graduation. It can also feel
overwhelming especially for new lab members, so we introduce new researchers to
the lab’s projects one at a time in order to help them learn about the projects,
navigate the IRB process, and let them choose where and how they want to
participate according to their interests and availability. It means keeping in
regular contact with weekly meetings for the different projects, with constant
reminders to take breaks and step back from the work in order to take care of
ourselves and each other. And, crucially, it must ensure that all researchers
(especially students!) are compensated and credited for their work in the forms
of hourly pay, presentations at conferences, and authorship on lab publications.
All of this continues to successfully sustain the lab after eight years with
minimal advertising (mostly by word of mouth in classes) because it draws people
in and gives them the space to explore their own research practices.
At the beginning of each semester, lab leads for each of the three major research
projects present broad semester goals. At the same time, if any group members
come up with a hypothesis, they are given a semester or two to play around with
the new approach. If the research idea seems sustainable, it becomes a long-term
project and is incorporated into a future beginning-of-semester goal
presentation. If not, we store detailed notes on the process in a “research questions” document for future reference. For
example, one of the undergraduate researchers got so excited about metaphors
that he spent a full semester mapping how often participants highlighted
metaphors and how tangible, familiar, or cliché the metaphors were. Although
this got stored away for a year, it turns out that it will be absolutely pivotal
as we start writing a new article (TBA) for a special issue of Narrative in 2023 titled “When Do
We Love Literature?: The Pleasures of Poetry Reading and the Power of
Story.”
Our projects range from fMRI studies of novel reading, to experiments on poetry
and pleasure, to cultural explorations of the stories we hear in music. Outside
the world of interdisciplinary experiments, the DHLC also organizes exhibitions
that blend art, medical humanities, and disability studies to create accessible
art installations in museums where everyone — including those along the visual
disability spectrum — can
touch the art. This means we have used DH
tools that range from computational software for analyzing brain data and
patterns in neural activation (FSL), text analysis tools such as Voyant and
sentiment analysis, and Guide by Cell, the latest web-based link that we use to
provide audio descriptions for Accessible Art visitors with visual disabilities
(much better than QR codes). The fact that we use DH tools so much in order to
do our research makes the lab an equally fascinating case study
in how digital tools blend the virtual and physical. Depending on the day and
number of computers, we use Skype, Zoom, or Google Hangouts, we write and edit
on Google Drive, and we communicate in almost twenty Slack channels for
different projects, though most researchers are active primarily in the few
related to their chosen projects. In the image above, a lost-in-sauce meme
motivated students to focus in the last frantic hours of poster writing before
our yearly undergraduate conference (see Figure 1). We use many of these same
channels to talk with collaborators around the world: Stanford University, New
York University, The University of Arkansas, Princeton University, and the
University of Hong Kong. In these ways, our lab is quite expansive, both
physically and virtually [
Perkel 2016].
This highly collaborative nature of our digital humanities research makes our lab
a “hybrid space,” something Steve Harrison and Paul Dourish
define as a space that “comprises both physical and
virtual”
[
Harrison 1996, 72]. We experience this physical–digital
interplay in the lab daily, in embodied interactions that are “framed simultaneously by the physical space, the virtual space
and the relationship between the two”
[
Harrison 1996, 72]. Interdisciplinary work in the lab
involves bridging not only disciplines but also places and practices, and how we
conduct research is in many ways defined and delimited by both physical and
virtual spaces (see Figure 2). For instance, our lab is located at Michigan
State University (MSU). Most of our lab members and participants therefore
reflect typical university demographics and range in ages from seventeen to
thirty-five. Thus, we constantly remind ourselves when analyzing data that we
have a particular group of college students who like coffee and poetry, are
interested in $5 Starbucks gift cards to participate in our latest study, and
have many other time commitments in their studies, work, and social lives. The
lab’s physical space comfortably fits only approximately ten of our thirty lab
members at a time. Accessibility measures are also ironically difficult in a lab
that intentionally organizes accessible art events — Natalie uses a rolling
walker when needed but can’t fit it inside our small space.
The role space plays in defining research becomes even more apparent when that
research involves placing people in circular magnetic tubes. As Joshua Mann
notes of digital technologies used to represent the past, nothing about this
present technology is “hermeneutically neutral”
[
Mann 2018]. The MRI scanner presents a panoply of physical
limitations on research almost too numerous to describe. Students cannot bring a
paperback inside a scanner, so they read a digital rendition of a
“page” (or sonnet) at the back of the scanner through a
mirror above their eyes, pushing a button or scrolling with a mouse to move
through the lines and pages.
Head movement in an MRI machine means the scan will pick up on the wrong brain
regions, so we make a participant as comfortable as possible, providing
ear-plugs to drown out the noise of pulse sequences, as well as foam pillows
under their knees, for their elbows, and alongside their head for comfort. In
this case, we accept the unique constraints on physical space inside an MRI
scanner (we can’t change Radiology...yet), but we use our humanist background to
push to the very edges of what’s traditionally allowed to be seen inside a
scanner. In a radical humanist departure from MRI tradition, our participants
read long sections of novels and poetry at their own pace, rather
than being presented with single sentences or phrases flashing rhythmically on
the screen. Yes, the physical constraints determine the research that can be
done. Yet DH can open new opportunities, as the DHLC’s first project
demonstrates.
The lab’s first project, “Jane Austen and the Neuroscience of
Reading and Attention,” was an fMRI study of the different levels of
attention we can bring to a novel and specifically the differences between close
and pleasure reading [
Phillips 2010]. Eighteen Ph.D. students in
English (picked for their literary training) were asked to read the first
chapters of Jane Austen’s
Mansfield Park inside an
MRI, switching between close reading and pleasure reading at defined sections.
Afterward, outside the scanner, they wrote informal literary essays on the
sections they’d close read. To our astonishment, they ended up quoting the text
extensively from memory [
Phillips 2015]
[
Phillips 2018]. Sponsored by an ACLS Digital Innovations grant,
we also began to map the unique patterns of quoting from Mansfield Park,
revealing an attraction to tangible and material words in the text and how these
mnemonic anchors grounded sophisticated analysis of class dynamics, stylistics,
gender roles, and Austen’s irony. Analysis is still ongoing, but this first
experiment exemplifies the spontaneity and delightful accident that marks all
our projects. In seeking to study the different levels of attention we bring to
books, we ended up being the first study of natural reading. Based on an article
gone viral, the study also ended up being an unexpected showcase for the value
of literary study, revealing the widespread brain regions activated by close
reading, a core skill in the liberal arts [
Goldman 2012]
[
Thompson 2012].
Ultimately, this study and the other DHLC projects we describe in the coming
pages reveal how digital humanities labs can take an active role in championing
the humanities in our communities. We believe that the interdisciplinary,
non-hierarchical structure of our lab is valuable philosophically, politically,
and practically. We see the different fields represented in the lab as equally
important partners in our work, rather than science chasing humanities or
humanities chasing science. In the same vein, the members of our lab are equally
important partners in our work, regardless of university rank. Together, these
approaches shake up the relationships between fields and between people, giving
everyone ownership in what the lab does and reminding us that everyone can and
should be part of the process of expanding human knowledge.
The Pleasures of Poetry Reading
We could say that the international recognition our first study
of Austen received naturally led to a second project, “Neuroaesthetics: Poetry and Aesthetic Pleasure.” We moved from
novels to poetry, conducting an experiment that emerged organically from a
research group centered at NYU on the neural and cognitive dimensions of
aesthetic experience. In reality, I was grading papers for a course on the
eighteenth-century novel with a friend when an unexpected email arrived from
a Dean at NYU declaring she and her colleagues had been following my
research and inviting me to join a global collaboration on neuroscience and
art. I knew if I wanted to take this on — that is, involve the lab in
designing three new experiments on literature, music, and art — I would need
to bring the lab with me. I said yes, and boarded a plane to New York… with
three students in tow.
What I didn’t expect was that it was absolutely necessary that
the students be there. Sitting at the table in a group of neuroscientific
leaders, I watched in awe as they offered insights (and even experimental
critiques) again and again, gaining the respect of famous scholars in the
neuroscience of music, vision, and art. Yet, as we all know, most of the
work — and many of the decisions — happens, not in the conference room, but
over dinner. During the wining and dining, the students mixed and mingled
with enviable ease, picking up comments that would prove invaluable. Had
they been virtually present over Skype, all that would have been lost. On
our final night, free from the formality of fine dining, we ordered to-go
Indian food and clustered over our laptops in my hotel room. As we pieced
together our notes from the weekend, and laughed over the comic tidbits we’d
overheard, we dropped all previous expectations and began to design a
totally new experiment on poetry — one that would advance Neuroaesthetics
and further define the DHLC. As we worked and laughed, I realized the lab
had been moved, so to speak, to a small hotel room in New York, with plastic
knives and chana masala in styrofoam cups.
Forging ahead with the energy from that first trip to NYU, and in ongoing
collaboration with G. Gabrielle Starr, we designed a study looking at real-time
responses to poetry [
Starr 2007]
[
Starr 2010]
[
Belfi 2018]. We wanted to capture readers’ aesthetic reactions to
poems as well as how their pleasure shifted as they read; thus, we asked
participants to highlight moments they found aesthetically pleasing and
displeasing while reading sonnets. First with pencil and paper, later with red
and green highlights on a computer screen, fifty MSU students registered their
aesthetic responses when reading a set of sonnets drawn from the early modern
period to now. After reading and highlighting, participants were asked to use a
provided numerical scale to rate the vividness of imagery, the poem’s positive
or negative overall theme, and the intensity of their own emotional response.
What we found fascinating is that, while individual tastes clearly differ, there
are stunning similarities in moments people collectively find moving. For
example, reading “Dawn in New York,” a sonnet by key
Harlem Renaissance writer Claude McKay, 60% of participants single out the
description of women and men wandering the streets of Manhattan with “wine-weakened” eyes as particularly aesthetically
pleasing [
McKay 2019]. At this point pleasure aligned across
readers, revealing an unexpected moment of cognitive and emotional unity.
The data for each sonnet was so rich, with so many things to explore, that
students involved in the poetry study decided to pursue two distinct research
questions. One group (nicknamed “
LIT”) is exploring the question of why so many people
“like” (or positively highlighted) obviously negative
words and phrases.
[2] They
observed, with a wry joy, that participants hadn’t only positively highlighted
moments of aesthetic pleasure associated with positive themes (i.e. love,
sunrises, hope) but also took pleasure in negative themes (i.e. war and
sadness). For instance, participants found poetic beauty in McKay’s “cheerless domes;” similarly, in Michael Ferris’ “The Catskill Eagle,” numerous participants loved the
moment where the sonnet describes “rage and wonder battle
cruel, and strange”
[
Ferris 2011]. The other group became fascinated with the question
of the potential differences between how English majors and non-English majors
read and enjoy poetry, with a zeal that earned them the group name “Oxford Comma.” Curious about the influence of a
literary education on aesthetic pleasure, they decided to design and run another
set of experiments. Fueled by coffee and candy from our famously always-full
candy pitcher and recently-donated Keurig machine, they ran another
approximately thirty psychology majors as participants (see Figure 3).
In the new experiments, Psychology majors highlighted what they loved (and
didn’t) about poetry, providing key data to match up with the previous data from
English majors. Immediately, we noticed radical differences in aesthetic
pleasure, with Psychology students generally loving Henry Wadsworth Longfellow
and English students delighting instead in the far lesser known poem, Catherine
Chandler’s “Henslow’s Sparrow.” Though Chandler’s
poem opens with extensive descriptions of the elusive bird, it turns, mid-poem,
to admit the poet has never heard or seen one:
Or so
they say; for I have yet to spy
the shy, elusive bird, or hear its song,
except in Audubon recordings. I
admit to shaky faith, but play along; and though my yard’s a skirl of
jays and crows,
someday it might show up. One never knows.
[Chandler 2019]
The part English majors love best is the
final cryptic phase: “One never knows.” In a world
where students notoriously complain that they don’t like poetry because they
don’t know what it means, the English majors are
drawn to the
uncertain ending that one can “never know [sic].”
What students highlighted as particularly pleasing (or not) grew more and more
riveting to us as we gathered more responses. Some phrases in poems stood out to
a majority of readers, either positively or negatively. But before we could
answer that, we had to be able to see it. We tried bolding words that
participants had repeatedly highlighted, but this method couldn’t represent the
magnitude of students who loved or hated a phrase. Based on the number of likes
we tried changing the font size by statistical increments but the rest of the
poem was too small to be read. The best answer we could find was to create a
statistical highlighting system where words and phrases got darker and darker,
in green for pleasure and red for displeasure, which worked well enough until we
reached a point where so many people liked or disliked a word that you could no
longer see it. Our lab solution was to just turn that word white (see Figure 4).
Fortunately, MSU is invested in DH beyond the lab, and had just created a
Digital Scholarship Lab in the library. We reached out with a rough idea in mind
to solve the visualization problem and they quickly set us up with someone who
knew a technology for heat mapping. Together, their GIS specialist and an
English undergrad from the lab solved the problem that we had been staring at
for a year using ArcGIS, a geographical mapping software. The result was a
heatmap that showed where participants collectively found the most pleasure and
displeasure in the sonnets they read. Amusingly, we later returned to the
Digital Scholarship Lab as invited presenters, with our co-created poetry heat
map on display. The presentation received many excited questions for our
innovative use of ArcGIS — a testament to how interdisciplinary scholarship can
generate new, often unexpected engagements with digital tools that push our
questions and how we answer them further.
The poetry study epitomizes the crucial role physical place and in-person
interactions play in advancing the work that goes on in and beyond the lab.
Being in the same physical space allows us to rapidly iterate on experiment
design, data analysis, and digital tool development in our studies, responding
to theoretical concerns and practical considerations as they emerge. However the
poetry study also demonstrates how the physical, the virtual, and the digital
are increasingly blurred in an interdisciplinary lab. As our most recent study
on music and narrative cognition reveals, online collaboration and digital tools
for both communication and community research are just as essential for
understanding how we encounter literary art in the twenty-first century.
Narrative Perceptions of Music
Some studies come from concerted effort and invited
collaboration, as at NYU. Others emerge by serendipitous accident. This was
certainly the case for our study on the stories we hear in music. I had gone
to Nashville with my fiancé for one of his (many) music cognition
conferences. I’d skipped out of most of the talks and spent much of my time
exploring the streets of Nashville, with its eccentric restaurants, huge
musical billboards, and fabulous collection of old bookstores. But one
poster session caught my eye, so I made my way back to the conference,
keeping my badge face-down to stay blissfully incognito. As I read over a
poster on “close listening” to music, I marveled at the
structural similarity to our Austen study’s exploration of close and
pleasure reading. All of a sudden, the presenter appeared and said: “Oh my goodness, it’s you!” I swiftly learned she
had designed her study at Stanford intentionally building off our previous
work. She had tons of questions and I was happy to help, delving into a
conversation that lasted far beyond the poster session and carried us down
the stairs toward lunch. I saw my fiancé deeply engaged in a conversation
about budget practicalities with the new treasurer for the society, and told
her we’d need to meet up later, exchanging cards. When she turned to me on
the last step and said, “I can’t believe we met. The
Brain on Jane!” the acoustics of the space magnified her voice so
that (it seemed) the whole room heard. Certainly, my fiance’s conversation
about treasury suddenly ended; his companion, Lisa, evidently familiar with
our study, promptly turned to me and said, “You’re a
cognitive narratologist? There’s something I need you to
see.”
Lisa Margulis, it turns out, is an interdisciplinary scholar who runs the
Music
Cognition Lab in the Music department at U. Arkansas (soon to be
moving to Princeton). She ran a study where she had participants listen to music
without words and asked them to describe any story they heard; a surprising
number of people did indeed hear a story [
Margulis 2017]. She sat
Natalie down and had her read some of the narratives they wrote, many of which
were incredibly similar, often sharing the same topics, themes, even specific
words. The images she showed Natalie — expanded and extended to represent the
full experiment — were to become for us the crucial narrative data to explore
convergences (and divergences) in the stories people hear in music. When Natalie
got back she called the lab leads together at the DHLC and asked the unaskable
question: “I know we have a lot going on at the lab — too
much already. I’m tired, you’re tired… still, is it interesting enough for
us to run with it anyway?” Everyone was fascinated. And, despite the
amount of work we had, it was too interesting to
not work with.
Supported by a number of fellowships (written together over Skype), an
ever-accumulating number of collaborators, and a National Science Foundation
(NSF) grant, we began to explore whether — particularly how and when — we use
narratives to understand music. In other words, when trying to make sense of a
new piece of music, do we begin to “hear” stories — or,
certain types of stories? How much do the tendencies to construct narratives
when listening to music, and to have those narratives align, depend on cultural
background? The study itself is deceptively simple: participants listen to a
piece of orchestral music and are asked if they imagined a story or elements of
a story while listening. If they say yes, they are asked to describe the
imagined story in as much detail as possible. The similarities in participant
answers were often startling. For example, almost 74% of participants listening
to an excerpt of Liszt’s music at U. Arkansas described a “cat-and-mouse story” (see Figure 5).
For the excerpt from Telemann’s “Suite in E Major,
II,” a completely different narrative emerged for listeners: 88% of
these subjects imagined wealthy — even royal — characters dancing in a fancy
ballroom. Unlike the cat-and-mouse stories, participants listening to Telemann
were highly focused on setting. According to their descriptions, any narrative
activities — dancing, feasting, socializing, etc. — took place, not
consecutively (i.e. cat chases mouse, mouse runs, cat chases again, etc...), but
recursively, choreographed in social tandem.
In order to assess how much cultural context affects these narratives, we are
partnering with researchers at the U. of Hong Kong to compare results of our
narrative listening studies at U. Arkansas and Michigan State with participants
from rural China. In this new cross-cultural version of the study, participants
from U. Arkansas, MSU,
and regions of rural China listen to both
Chinese and Western music, allowing us to explore if cultural familiarity with a
certain musical style affects the likelihood of imagining stories, or the
similarity of the stories they tell. Through five iterations of this study, from
the original at U. Arkansas to the ongoing research in China, we have data from
a total of 1202 participants. After Natalie’s in-person meeting with Lisa, the
rest of the NSF
Narrative Perceptions of Music
study unfolded in virtual space. The musical selections are stored online and
played for participants on a computer, with participants’ responses stored
carefully in coded Excel files. Since our collaboration spans the globe as well
as the country, most of our conversations happen online, fitting the trend of
increasingly digital scholarly communication [
Lopez 2015].
Messages and results wing back and forth between MSU, U. Arkansas, and the U. of
Hong Kong. Most often, we use Zoom, Google Hangouts or Skype to create a
collaborative, interdisciplinary, and cross-institutional space to allow
ethno-musicologists, computational sound-technicians, cognitive musicologists
and narrative theorists to connect. Bringing a variety of DH and humanistic
tools to the analysis of these narratives, we are beginning to explore how much
these music-inspired storylines converge or diverge for each piece — with the
goal of testing this across cultures. This means that at any given time, walking
into the lab, you might find people dispersed, each working on a different
computer and using a different DH tool to explore similarities and points of
uniqueness in our now ~1,500 musical stories. At one computer, we have a student
(relatively new to the lab) using Voyant to offer a first-round exploration of
common themes in the seventy-nine stories from the students at U. Arkansas
listening to musical excerpt WA15. With appropriate stop words, we can begin to
see what stands out as a through-line for the stories linked to this particular
piece. Do people tend to invoke pirates? Dancing squirrels? Star Wars and/or
lightsabers? Out of the word-cloud, we can then return relatively quickly to the
specific stories and check their context for more nuance and richness, always
keeping a humanities eye out for themes that might go unnoticed by sheer word
count. Comments jotted down on a sticky note pose questions: 1) Do these
commonly-used words actually point us to themes? 2) Does sheer frequency of a
phrase reveal focus? 3) How about what’s implied or left unsaid here...which
often seems to carry as much meaning as the explicit and stated?!?
If you look next to her, on the same side of the lab is an English major, who
jumped into our poetry study so early and deeply that he taught himself E-Prime,
the standard tool for creating computerized experimental design and data
collection. Now, having finished designing macros for our lab computers to make
certain common lab tasks easier, and having finished developing an algorithm to
map word count for our ever-increasing data-set of stories that people heard in
music, we find him deep in network analysis, looking to see how often key terms
and phrases from the music-inspired storylines intersected for different pieces
of music. He’s never lost his English side — you should hear him go on about
metaphor theory some day.
Across the lab, with headphones in and deeply focused, is a tech-savvy grad
student in English and DH developing a tool in R to improve sentiment analysis
for our narratives, seeking to assess subtle similarities and differences in the
mood or tone of the stories that might otherwise
go unnoticed. In a previous effort to do this for ~500 stories with sentiment
analysis tools from IBM Watson and Stanford NLP, two dogged undergrads ran into
problems so obvious we couldn’t continue without digging into the algorithm. Our
funniest example was about ducks. As it turned out both IBM Watson and Stanford
NLP rated a student-imagined story about a bunch of ducks swimming in a pond
gathering food as “strongly negative.” Other less
comic cases of this abounded, and, knowing that we must be thinking critically
about these tools, we decided, “why not just create our
own.” Thus, our brilliant grad student developing a tool in R to
better analyze the sentiment of our narratives and capture moods that will
always slip by word count.
At the end of the day, no matter how many different tools we’re working with (or
how many people are at the lab), someone usually jokingly admits that candy
isn’t cutting it. “I’m going to need a drink after this…
does anyone want to join?” and off we go to a local bar or
restaurant. We stay away from work as much as we can at the bar but conversation
often includes laughing over the inside jokes and funniest stories we’ve seen.
Some of our favorites examples include: “I imagined a fight
between two squirrels,”
“This music sounded like a mixture of Michael Myers and
clowns,” and “plz stp playing songs that stress
me out.” While all of these last examples happened
in
(or near) the lab,
Narrative Perceptions of Music
is one of the best studies for talking about how extensive a role virtual tools
play in DHLC life, especially considering the national and international
collaborations. At one point we met with Lisa virtually on Google Hangouts to
discuss challenges with the system we were developing for categorizing the
narratives. With the help of our new technological tool, OWL, we could have 360
degree video conference camera scope that allowed access to the whole room while
automatically shifting focus to different people as they spoke. Still better,
everyone in the room could hear (and be heard) while simultaneously being seen
no matter where they were. This technology helped the conversation stay vibrant
and productive, bringing people together over great distance [
Bly 1993]. Modeling the spirit of the lab, Lisa and Devin (two
full professors) listened intently as students offered critical feedback on how
literary scholars would approach narrative categorization. For instance, when
they offered the example “I imagined animated shadows
creeping over the walls” as an abstract narrative without characters
or agents, students were able to offer feedback as equals. When our
undergraduate lab lead pointed out that shadows (as well as colors, objects,
animals, etc.) could be considered agents (i.e. characters), traditional
professorial power dynamics went out the window. Lisa simply said “I trust your intuition as a literary scholar.”
It is this same spirit of equality and open-access that motivates our development
of
Adagia, an online app that collects
and maps the narratives of users who listen to a short musical excerpt and share
any story they heard (see Figure 6). Their response is immediately visible to
them (and others) on a GPS-based map, allowing us to explore how local and
cultural influences shape the stories. Our long-term goal for Adagia is to allow
users to enrich and reshape the project (adding new music samples online,
suggesting new questions, or proposing an easier coding platform). Crowdsourcing
traditional scientific research, rendering it online, and making it
humanities-friendly, this digital work will also yield an ever-expanding set of
stories ripe for DH text analytics. We view Adagia as filling a similar role to
Amazon’s Mechanical Turk, an online platform often used to scale up psychology
research, but without the corporate overlords or problematic labor practices
[
Burhmester 2011]
[
Bohannon 2016]. The virtual and digital elements of our work
allow us to reimagine our situated research practices and bring our work beyond
the DHLC, MSU, and China.
Neurodiversity, Medical Humanities, and Accessible Art
In Lab Girl, Hope Jahren has a beautiful and moving description
of her lab space: My laboratory is a place where the
lights are always on. My laboratory has no windows, but it needs none.
It is self-contained. It is its own world. My lab is both private and
familiar, populated by a small number of people who know one another
well. My lab is the place where I put my brain out on my fingers and I
do things….In my lab, whatever I need is greatly outbalanced by what I
have. The drawers are packed full with items that might come in handy.
Every object in my lab — no matter how small or misshapen — exists for a
reason, even if its purpose has not yet been found….The door is locked
and I know everyone who has a key. Because the outside world cannot come
into the lab, the lab has become the place where I can be the real me.
(18-19)
I find this gorgeous because it captures such a strong part of
how I feel about the DHLC. Yet there’s a part of the lab that’s different;
something central to its beauty. Yes, the lab is its own world and a
sanctuary to me. Yes, it’s stocked with objects whose purpose may not have
been found (yet). But every time I go into the lab, I see both people who I
know well and people I’ve never met before. And it’s just as often the
people I’ve never seen before who have suddenly (I learn) solved an
experimental problem — or a problem with complex software and coding — I
didn’t even know existed. This shows what it means to be an
“open” humanities lab. We have IRB to protect the
confidentiality of brain data, but since the IRB training is essential and
free, the lab is radically and powerfully open. This means that problems are
solved from the ground up, not top-down. And that the students come up with
new questions to ask about each and every project we do, with an energy that
carries it far beyond the original idea.
The ultimate proof of this came when I had to go down briefly
for medical leave. I’ll admit, egocentrically, I worried that the lab would
come to a complete and grinding halt without me. Instead, it flourished as
much it always has, possibly even more, since the students knew, deeply now,
that what I’d been telling them — that they had the leadership, they had to
trust their own instincts, and they could take it forward — was true. They’d
been trained for it. This is the ultimate proof that the lab is not
vertically driven, nor hierarchical by nature; it continued and prospered
while the director was down. Two English majors from a class I taught who
joined the lab while I was away ended up, in their first semester, designing
and running an entire new branch of the poetry study, getting scientists to
highlight the aspects of the poems they found most powerful (or not).
Another group went out into the dormitories and hallways and gathered
everyday students’ definitions of aesthetic pleasure. Perhaps most
impressively, an Accessible Art event that I’d been working on, pulled out
of my hands by necessity, expanded based on student innovation to include
not just students — as had been my original model — but local community
artists and a week-long public exhibition. While away, I found myself
missing the lab more than I can say; going in to the DHLC had always meant
going into a space where things were happening...where students always had
new projects and ideas (not that three experiments and an accessible art
project weren’t enough to keep us on our toes). Not that I’m back, I realize
what I missed most; the vibrance in the atmosphere — and the sanctuary it
provided, always showing, rather than telling that humanists and scientists
could do things together...at times, brilliantly...and in ways that expand
beyond experiments into the community.
One of the most powerful ways the DHLC breaks the mold of what labs usually do
(obscure experiments behind closed doors) is through a series of multisensory
and inclusive art exhibits that have come to be known as Accessible Art. In 2015
the student editor of
Exceptions, a journal housed
within the DHLC that publishes work by and for people with visual disabilities,
approached an English professor and an art professor with the idea for a
touchable art exhibit. English students would select or create a sensory-rich
passage of text, which students in the art class would use as inspiration for a
multisensory piece of art. The Accessible Art exhibit was born with the motto
“Touch the Art!” After that first year,
Exceptions editors continued to plan different
accessible artistic events, including a second Accessible Art exhibit in 2017.
In honor of Veteran’s day this exhibit focused on depictions of war, and
drew
over 200 visitors. One wrote, “This changed the
paradigm of art for me, before this event I only thought of art as visual
and did not consider the fact that art can and should engage all senses and
be accessible for all people.” Another put it this way: “it was actually accessible to everyone. QR codes were
available to have the literary aspects of the art read aloud and then of
course we were encouraged to touch the art, which is something unheard of in
a museum.” Our events were never only about opening museum spaces to
visitors with visual disabilities, important as such work is. Instead, we sought
to encourage every visitor to think about engaging with art in a different way.
The enjoyment that comes from the multi-sensory indulgence of touching the
untouchable opens people’s minds to consider what a more inclusive and
accessible world might look like (see Figure 7).
It was precisely this call for art to engage all senses and all people that
guided our vision for the 2019 exhibit. Natalie was on medical leave, but it
didn’t occur to us to question whether a group of students could pull off the
project ourselves — our collective years with the lab had taught us we could. We
began with the pattern of the two previous exhibits, and in August 2018 a few
graduate students attended a folk art festival and met a local quiltmaker. Her
fidget quilts — lap blankets with tassels, beads, textural patterns, zippers,
and ribbons — give people with Alzheimer’s and dementia something to play with
that soothes the restlessness than often accompanies those diseases (see Figure
8).
We wanted her art as part of the exhibit, and started dreaming of an event that
brought together student and community art. The project grew to include almost a
dozen undergraduate and graduate students on the planning team as well as nearly
fifty undergraduate students and community members creating multi-sensory art,
designing the exhibit, documenting the exhibit’s accessibility, writing grants,
and organizing tours for local schools. In addition to the fidget quilt, our art
pieces included other textiles, like a piece of embroidery that uses both
Braille and the Latin alphabet. Another installation was a mirror hung at forty
inches from the ground, the proper level for someone in a wheelchair, surrounded
by selfies the artist has taken of herself in mirrors around the MSU campus; the
pictures are mostly the top of her head and empty space, because the mirrors are
too high. Visitors were encouraged to take selfies in this mirror — people who
don’t use wheelchairs will mostly get torso shots, demonstrating what it feels
like when the world is not set up for you. Numerous sculptures played with
texture, like one that is mixed metal and ceramics, another that incorporates
moss, and a third that is a window box full of fake flowers created from
materials like yarn, tulle, pipe cleaners, and feathers.
The 2019 Accessible Art exhibit is a testament to the way the DHLC empowers its
members not only to take on independent work but also to take it outside the lab
itself. Our “closed” lab is just one node in an open,
interconnected web that stretches across the campus, country, and world, similar
to other emerging art labs [
Pormann 2015].
What’s in a Lab? Digital Humanities, Interdisciplinary Futures
The brief sketches of the DHLC and its projects, students, and
possibilities that we’ve included here are only the beginning of where our
lab has been, and where it is going. It certainly hasn’t always been easy to
organize and run an interdisciplinary DH lab, but thankfully I’ve had the
support of many fantastic colleagues — including my students — in doing so.
In so many ways, the DHLC is driven by student innovation and research,
including students creating their own studies using the skills and tools
they learned in the lab. For example, graduate students Cody Mejeur, Soohyun
Cho, and Jes Lopez have designed experiments that identify the neural
networks involved in narrative comprehension in video games, investigate the
cognitive and cultural effects of Kindle Popular Highlights, and analyze the
representations of autistic characters in literature and film through
embedded sentiment, respectively.
If I could sum up the most important lessons I’ve learned along
the way for those interested in building and running similar labs, these
would be my main points: - Involve students as equals. Not just as
students but as lively, active, curious people. Let them explore and
grow. Even better, treat them with the respect you give your
colleagues and global collaborators. You’ll be astonished.
- Accept that serendipitous accidents will grow into
new things: a lab, an fMRI, a poetry study, or a series of
accessible art exhibits. Embrace it, and embrace the fact that each
new project will make you want a new tool.
- Things expand: be ready (it’s going to be
okay). The lab can’t hold it all, so reserve extra rooms.
- Think of interdisciplinary as pluralism. It’s
not DH vs traditional methods, but rather a both/and situation that
involves a lot of translation work, over and over again.
- Allow yourself the joy of working with
students. Running a lab is a lot of work, I won’t lie. But
you’ll experience moments of personal fulfillment and pride in a
group that are absolutely unrivaled.
In the Chronicle of Higher
Education, Jonathan Kramnick claims we are living in an academic
moment governed by “The Interdisciplinary
Delusion”
[Kramnick 2018]. And there are days when it does feel like a
delusion, but not the one that Kramnick cites. It feels like a delusion when
on the morning of a university-wide research competition, I stop by the lab
to see students in English, Biology, and Education still adding new data to
their posters, and then, hours later, watch them accept the top prize. When
I return from medical leave to discover that students have run two new
studies despite joining the lab just a month earlier. When I step into the
Broad Art Museum for the lab’s Accessible Art exhibition and get to touch my
first work of student art and feel how they captured an element of the
poetry. It’s the brilliance and energy of the students that are drawn to
this work, not the facade of interdisciplinary novelty, that so astonishes
and inspires me.
Interdisciplinary work is a shift of view, not a truth claim.
No one in the lab and none of my colleagues feel that we will somehow find
the absolute truth about poetry and pleasure. Far from it. Instead we are
simply using a different set of tools to get a glimpse into what 30 students
love, or hate, about sixteen sonnets. Our music and narrative study may
extend to China, but it has the same limits. The excitement isn’t all “an interdisciplinary fantasy of the moment.” It
comes from real students, professors, and colleagues around the world who
have their own deep disciplinary training and are contributing it to
illuminate a new facet that otherwise would never have been seen about how
we engage with literature, music, and art.
The common fear of digital humanities and other interdisciplinary fields like
literary neuroscience is that we are attempting to scientize the humanities,
making them more quantifiable, measurable, and profitable [
Pask 2018]. Without wading too deeply into a longstanding debate
about the validity of these fields, we want to acknowledge that there are
reasons to be cautious about a type of interdisciplinary research that just
seeks to import scientific knowledge to the humanities in order to attract more
institutional support and grant dollars [
Allington 2016]
[
Kirschenbaum 2016]
[
Singh 2016]. In the DHLC, we make it a primary goal to challenge
this trend. We believe that truly interdisciplinary work isn’t a one-way street,
as though the sciences have the authority on truth and reality and the
humanities are here merely to dress it up. Rather, we are proud of having a lab
space that humanitizes the sciences, seeing young neuroscientists regularly
reading literature and poetry, and introducing scholars in English, Education,
Rhetoric, and other areas to neuroscience and digital tools for analyzing
artistic media. Interdisciplinary digital humanities labs are uniquely
positioned to make these points of contact happen, and to do the critical work of
championing the humanities and resisting the institutional privileging of the
sciences.
In an age where human life is increasingly shaped by digital media — indeed,
where we seem to be evolving alongside our technology, as N. Katherine Hayles
argues — brain imaging technology can give us new insight on how human
meaning-making is embodied, and how it operates in different digital contexts
[
Hayles 2012]. At the same time, our ability to critique the
forms, power, and ideologies that shape the sciences we use helps us to
encounter the limits of neuroscience — all the many places where existing
studies fail to account for the rich diversity of human experience [
Kim 2018]. The implications of such understandings are potentially
far-reaching. For example, knowing how we close read and pleasure read
differently can change how we teach and approach texts, or learning how we
perceive music as narrative can affect how we compose and use everything from
advertising jingles to symphonies.
Our work in the digital humanities and literary cognition is just one of many
possible sites for interdisciplinary collaboration and research. Imagine, for
example, what humanities-based ecocriticism labs might look like, or consider
what the humanities could bring to the critical coding and computing centers and
networks that have sprung up in recent years [
Ruiz 2017].
[3] In many ways we have only begun to see the potential
combinations and new perspectives that such lab work can generate. And even on
the level of individual labs, as the DHLC demonstrates, work is expanding beyond
the walls of any one institution into many physical and virtual spaces across
the globe [
Klein 2016]. Approaches from the DHLC have inspired the
structure and process of other digital humanities labs, like Dr. Stephen
Grandchamp’s
Digital Humanities Lab at
the University of Maine at Farmington, which utilizes the student-led model of
the DHLC by having undergraduates work with faculty to integrate DH into
curricula and to provide one-on-one instruction for students. Grandchamp also
adjusted and adapted this model in ways that are different from how the DHLC
run: all his student assistants have been hired as freshmen with the goal of
working four complete years in the lab. This creative reconfiguration has led to
brilliant work for DH outreach in Maine — the student assistants are taking the
lead on a digital archival project by scanning the historical catalogs
(including creative writing and visual art) of a local seed cooperative with the
goal of creating a fully-searchable online archive.
Of course there are good reasons to be suspicious of these trends, and thankfully
the humanities draw our attention to questions of access, privilege, and power
in pursuing them [
Lothian 2013]
[
Risam 2015]. Still, if there is anything we have learned that can
help others in running, starting, or just considering a lab, it is that
embracing a “try everything” ethos can lead to unexpected and
generative opportunities for changing how we think, especially when trying
everything is inclusive of different peoples and supportive of their skills,
knowledges, and goals. Our research practices are always situated within various
types of institutions, traditions, and systems of power, but they also always
hold out the possibility for new and revolutionary insights to spontaneously
emerge. To be more specific to the digital humanities, if we are to critically
engage with digital technologies, then we have to be willing to let those
engagements evolve and emerge to address the critical question or situation at
hand. We also have to be open to taking risks, and even to failing. After all,
we can learn just as much (and sometimes more!) from the experiments that fail
as the ones that produce promising results. The great benefit of prioritizing
the humanities in this trial and error is that they can guide the process,
minimizing the harm and emphasizing the importance of considering the meanings
and ethics behind research projects. This interdisciplinary work is not the
delusion of one moment in higher education, it is an ongoing project that
contributes to answering some of the most pernicious problems of the
twenty-first century.
Works Cited
Allington 2016 Allington, D., S. Brouillette,
and D. Golumbia. “Neoliberal Tools (and Archives): A
Political History of Digital Humanities”. Los
Angeles Review of Books. May 1, 2016.
Belfi 2018 Belfi, A. M., E. A. Vessel, and G. G.
Starr. “Individual ratings of vividness predict aesthetic
appeal in poetry”. Psychology of Aesthetics,
Creativity, and the Arts 12 (3): 341–350.
Bly 1993 Bly, S. A., S. R. Harrison, and S. Irwin.
“Media spaces: bringing people together in a video,
audio, and computing environment”. Communications of the ACM 36 (1): 28–46.
Bohannon 2016 Bohannon, J. “Mechanical Turk upends social sciences”. Science, 352 (6291): 1263–1264.
Burhmester 2011 Buhrmester, M., T. Kwang, and
S. D. Gosling. “Amazon’s Mechanical Turk: A New Source of
Inexpensive, Yet High-Quality, Data?”
Perspectives on Psychological Science, 6 (1):
3–5.
Harrison 1996 Harrison, S., and P. Dourish.
“Re-place-ing space: the roles of place and space in
collaborative systems”. Proceedings of the 1996
ACM conference on Computer supported cooperative work - CSCW ’96.
Boston: ACM Press, 1996.
Hayles 2012 Hayles, K. How we think: digital media and contemporary
technogenesis. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press,
2012.
Kim 2018 Kim, S. J. “Sense and
Sensitivity: Cognitive Approaches to Race & Ethnicity”. 2018
International Conference on Narrative, Montreal, Quebec, CA, April 2018.
Klein 2016 Klein, L. F., and M.K. Gold. “Digital Humanities: The Expanded Field”. Debates in the Digital Humanities 2016. Edited by L.
F. Klein and M. K. Gold, University of Minnesota Press, 2016.
Lopez 2015 Lopez, A., F. Rowland, and K.
Fitzpatrick. “On Scholarly Communication and the Digital
Humanities: An Interview with Kathleen Fitzpatrick”. In The Library With The Lead Pipe. 2015.
Lothian 2013 Lothian, A., and A. Phillips. “Can Digital Humanities Mean Transformative Critique?”
Journal of E-Media Studies, 3 (1).
Mann 2018 Mann, J. L. “How
Technology Means: Texts, History, and Their Associated Technologies”.
Digital Humanities Quarterly, 12 (3).
Mann 2019 Mann, R. “Paid to Do
but Not to Think: Reevaluating the Role of Graduate Student
Collaborators”. Debates in the Digital
Humanities 2019. Edited by L. F. Klein and M. K. Gold, University of
Minnesota Press, 2019.
Margulis 2017 Margulis, E. H. “An Exploratory Study of Narrative Experiences of
Music”. Music Perception: An Interdisciplinary Journal , 35 (2):
235–248.
Pask 2018 Pask, K. “In the
Literature Lab”. Modern Language
Quarterly, 79 (1): 1–24.
Pawlicka 2017 Pawlicka, U. “Data, Collaboration, Laboratory: Bringing Concepts from Science into
Humanities Practice”. English Studies,
98 (5): 526–541.
Perkel 2016 Perkel, J. M. “How
scientists use Slack”. Nature, 541
(7635): 123–124.
Phillips 2010 Phillips, N. “Distraction as Liveliness of Mind: A Cognitive Approach to Characterization
in Jane Austen”. Theory of Mind and
Literature. Edited by P. Leverage, H. Mancing, J. Marston William,
and R. Schweickert, Purdue University Press, 2010.
Phillips 2015 Phillips, N. “Literary Neuroscience and History of Mind: An Interdisciplinary fMRI Study
of Attention and Jane Austen”. The Oxford
Handbook of Cognitive Literary Studies. Edited by L. Zunshine,
Oxford University Press, 2015.
Phillips 2018 Phillips, N., S. Antonucci, L.
Grasser, M. Meter, R. Narayan, K. Persenaire, K. Smith, S. Smith, and L.
Straley. “Patterns of Attention and Memory in Jane Austen:
Literary Neuroscience, Intellectual History, and the Importance of
Individual Difference”.Jane Austen and sciences
of the mind. Edited by B. Lau, London: Routledge, 2018.
Pormann 2015 Pormann, P. E. “Interdisciplinarity: Inside Manchester’s ‘arts lab’”. Nature, 525: 318–319.
Risam 2015 Risam, R. “Beyond
the Margins: Intersectionality and the Digital Humanitie””. Digital Humanities Quarterly, 9 (2).
Ruiz 2017 Ruiz, S., M. Areguin, E. Estrada, J.
Jimenez, D. Lopez, K. Sanchez, and J. Valenzuela. “Radicalizing the Digital Humanities: Reimagining Environmental Justice
Research and Teaching”. Radical Teacher,
109 (1): 29–37.
Starr 2007 Starr, G. G. “Poetic
Subjects and Grecian Urns: Close Reading and the Tools of Cognitive
Science”. Modern Philology, 105 (1):
48–61.
Starr 2010 Starr, G. G. “Multisensory Imagery”. Introduction to
Cognitive Cultural Studies. Edited by L. Zunshine, John Hopkins
University Press, 2010.
Wythoff 2018 Wythoff, G. “On
Method in the Humanities”. Configurations, 26 (3): 289–295.